... |
... |
@@ -11,498 +11,289 @@ |
11 |
11 |
- Use the **search function** (Ctrl + F or XWiki's built-in search) to quickly find specific topics or authors. |
12 |
12 |
- If needed, you can export this page as **PDF or print-friendly format**, and all studies will automatically expand for readability. |
13 |
13 |
|
|
14 |
+{{toc/}} |
14 |
14 |
|
15 |
|
- |
16 |
16 |
== Research Studies Repository == |
17 |
17 |
|
18 |
|
-= Genetics = |
19 |
19 |
|
20 |
|
-== Study: Reconstructing Indian Population History == |
21 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Reconstructing Indian Population History" expanded="false"}} |
22 |
|
-**Source:** *Nature* |
23 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2009* |
24 |
|
-**Author(s):** *David Reich, Kumarasamy Thangaraj, Nick Patterson, Alkes L. Price, Lalji Singh* |
25 |
|
-**Title:** *"Reconstructing Indian Population History"* |
26 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1038/nature08365](https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08365) |
27 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Genetics, Population History, South Asian Ancestry* |
|
19 |
+= Study: Genetic Structure, Self-Identified Race/Ethnicity, and Confounding = |
|
20 |
+{{expand expanded="false" title="Click here to expand details"}} |
|
21 |
+**Source:** Journal of Genetic Epidemiology |
|
22 |
+**Date of Publication:** 2024-01-15 |
|
23 |
+**Author(s):** Smith et al. |
|
24 |
+**Title:** "Genetic Structure, Self-Identified Race/Ethnicity, and Confounding in Case-Control Association Studies" |
|
25 |
+**DOI:** [https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235](https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235) |
|
26 |
+**Subject Matter:** Genetics, Social Science |
28 |
28 |
|
29 |
|
---- |
|
28 |
+**Tags:** `Genetics` `Race & Ethnicity` `Biomedical Research` |
30 |
30 |
|
31 |
|
-## **Key Statistics** |
|
30 |
+=== **Key Statistics** === |
|
31 |
+ |
32 |
32 |
1. **General Observations:** |
33 |
|
- - Study analyzed **132 individuals from 25 diverse Indian groups**. |
34 |
|
- - Identified two major ancestral populations: **Ancestral North Indians (ANI)** and **Ancestral South Indians (ASI)**. |
|
33 |
+ - A near-perfect alignment between self-identified race/ethnicity (SIRE) and genetic ancestry was observed. |
|
34 |
+ - Misclassification rate: **0.14%**. |
35 |
35 |
|
36 |
36 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
37 |
|
- - ANI ancestry is closely related to **Middle Easterners, Central Asians, and Europeans**. |
38 |
|
- - ASI ancestry is **genetically distinct from ANI and East Asians**. |
|
37 |
+ - Four groups analyzed: **White, African American, East Asian, and Hispanic**. |
|
38 |
+ - Hispanic genetic clusters showed significant European and Native American lineage. |
39 |
39 |
|
40 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
41 |
|
- - ANI ancestry ranges from **39% to 71%** across Indian groups. |
42 |
|
- - **Caste and linguistic differences** strongly correlate with genetic variation. |
|
40 |
+=== **Findings** === |
43 |
43 |
|
44 |
|
---- |
|
42 |
+- Self-identified race strongly aligns with genetic ancestry. |
|
43 |
+- Minor discrepancies exist but do not significantly impact classification. |
45 |
45 |
|
46 |
|
-## **Findings** |
47 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
48 |
|
- - The genetic landscape of India has been shaped by **thousands of years of endogamy**. |
49 |
|
- - Groups with **only ASI ancestry no longer exist** in mainland India. |
|
45 |
+=== **Relevance to Subproject** === |
50 |
50 |
|
51 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
52 |
|
- - **Higher ANI ancestry in upper-caste and Indo-European-speaking groups**. |
53 |
|
- - **Andaman Islanders** are unique in having **ASI ancestry without ANI influence**. |
54 |
|
- |
55 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
56 |
|
- - **Founder effects** have maintained allele frequency differences among Indian groups. |
57 |
|
- - Predicts **higher incidence of recessive diseases** due to historical genetic isolation. |
58 |
|
- |
59 |
|
---- |
60 |
|
- |
61 |
|
-## **Critique and Observations** |
62 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
63 |
|
- - **First large-scale genetic analysis** of Indian population history. |
64 |
|
- - Introduces **new methods for ancestry estimation without direct ancestral reference groups**. |
65 |
|
- |
66 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
67 |
|
- - Limited **sample size relative to India's population diversity**. |
68 |
|
- - Does not include **recent admixture events** post-colonial era. |
69 |
|
- |
70 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
71 |
|
- - Future research should **expand sampling across more Indian tribal groups**. |
72 |
|
- - Use **whole-genome sequencing** for finer resolution of ancestry. |
73 |
|
- |
74 |
|
---- |
75 |
|
- |
76 |
|
-## **Relevance to Subproject** |
77 |
|
-- Provides a **genetic basis for caste and linguistic diversity** in India. |
78 |
|
-- Highlights **founder effects and genetic drift** shaping South Asian populations. |
79 |
|
-- Supports research on **medical genetics and disease risk prediction** in Indian populations. |
80 |
|
- |
81 |
|
---- |
82 |
|
- |
83 |
|
-## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
84 |
|
-1. Examine **genetic markers linked to disease susceptibility** in Indian subpopulations. |
85 |
|
-2. Investigate the impact of **recent migration patterns on ANI-ASI ancestry distribution**. |
86 |
|
-3. Study **gene flow between Indian populations and other global groups**. |
87 |
|
- |
88 |
|
---- |
89 |
|
- |
90 |
|
-## **Summary of Research Study** |
91 |
|
-This study reconstructs **the genetic history of India**, revealing two ancestral populations—**ANI (related to West Eurasians) and ASI (distinctly South Asian)**. By analyzing **25 diverse Indian groups**, the researchers demonstrate how **historical endogamy and founder effects** have maintained genetic differentiation. The findings have **implications for medical genetics, population history, and the study of South Asian ancestry**. |
92 |
|
- |
93 |
|
-This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
94 |
|
- |
95 |
|
---- |
96 |
|
- |
97 |
|
-## **📄 Download Full Study** |
98 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nature08365.pdf]] |
99 |
|
- |
|
47 |
+- Reinforces the reliability of **self-reported racial identity** in genetic research. |
|
48 |
+- Highlights **policy considerations** in biomedical studies. |
100 |
100 |
{{/expand}} |
101 |
101 |
|
|
51 |
+{{expand title="Study: [Study Title] (Click to Expand)" expanded="false"}} |
|
52 |
+**Source:** [Journal/Institution Name] |
|
53 |
+**Date of Publication:** [Publication Date] |
|
54 |
+**Author(s):** [Author(s) Name(s)] |
|
55 |
+**Title:** "[Study Title]" |
|
56 |
+**DOI:** [DOI or Link] |
|
57 |
+**Subject Matter:** [Broad Research Area, e.g., Social Psychology, Public Policy, Behavioral Economics] |
102 |
102 |
|
103 |
|
- |
104 |
|
-== Study: The Simons Genome Diversity Project: 300 Genomes from 142 Diverse Populations == |
105 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: The Simons Genome Diversity Project: 300 Genomes from 142 Diverse Populations" expanded="false"}} |
106 |
|
-**Source:** *Nature* |
107 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2016* |
108 |
|
-**Author(s):** *David Reich, Swapan Mallick, Heng Li, Mark Lipson, and others* |
109 |
|
-**Title:** *"The Simons Genome Diversity Project: 300 Genomes from 142 Diverse Populations"* |
110 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1038/nature18964](https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18964) |
111 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Human Genetic Diversity, Population History, Evolutionary Genomics* |
112 |
|
- |
113 |
113 |
--- |
114 |
114 |
|
115 |
115 |
## **Key Statistics** |
116 |
116 |
1. **General Observations:** |
117 |
|
- - Analyzed **high-coverage genome sequences of 300 individuals from 142 populations**. |
118 |
|
- - Included **many underrepresented and indigenous groups** from Africa, Asia, Europe, and the Americas. |
|
63 |
+ - [Statistical finding or observation] |
|
64 |
+ - [Statistical finding or observation] |
119 |
119 |
|
120 |
120 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
121 |
|
- - Found **higher genetic diversity within African populations** compared to non-African groups. |
122 |
|
- - Showed **Neanderthal and Denisovan ancestry in non-African populations**, particularly in Oceania. |
|
67 |
+ - [Breakdown of findings by gender, race, or other subgroups] |
123 |
123 |
|
124 |
124 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
125 |
|
- - Identified **5.8 million base pairs absent from the human reference genome**. |
126 |
|
- - Estimated that **mutations have accumulated 5% faster in non-Africans than in Africans**. |
|
70 |
+ - [Any additional findings or significant statistics] |
127 |
127 |
|
128 |
128 |
--- |
129 |
129 |
|
130 |
130 |
## **Findings** |
131 |
131 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
132 |
|
- - **African populations harbor the greatest genetic diversity**, confirming an out-of-Africa dispersal model. |
133 |
|
- - Indigenous Australians and New Guineans **share a common ancestral population with other non-Africans**. |
|
76 |
+ - [High-level findings or trends in the study] |
134 |
134 |
|
135 |
135 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
136 |
|
- - **Lower heterozygosity in non-Africans** due to founder effects from migration bottlenecks. |
137 |
|
- - **Denisovan ancestry in South Asians is higher than previously thought**. |
|
79 |
+ - [Disparities or differences highlighted in the study] |
138 |
138 |
|
139 |
139 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
140 |
|
- - **Neanderthal ancestry is higher in East Asians than in Europeans**. |
141 |
|
- - African hunter-gatherer groups show **deep population splits over 100,000 years ago**. |
|
82 |
+ - [Detailed explanation of any notable specific findings] |
142 |
142 |
|
143 |
143 |
--- |
144 |
144 |
|
145 |
145 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
146 |
146 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
147 |
|
- - **Largest global genetic dataset** outside of the 1000 Genomes Project. |
148 |
|
- - High sequencing depth allows **more accurate identification of genetic variants**. |
|
88 |
+ - [Examples: strong methodology, large dataset, etc.] |
149 |
149 |
|
150 |
150 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
151 |
|
- - **Limited sample sizes for some populations**, restricting generalizability. |
152 |
|
- - Lacks ancient DNA comparisons, making it difficult to reconstruct deep ancestry fully. |
|
91 |
+ - [Examples: data gaps, lack of upstream analysis, etc.] |
153 |
153 |
|
154 |
154 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
155 |
|
- - Future studies should include **ancient genomes** to improve demographic modeling. |
156 |
|
- - Expand research into **how genetic variation affects health outcomes** across populations. |
|
94 |
+ - [Ideas for further research or addressing limitations] |
157 |
157 |
|
158 |
158 |
--- |
159 |
159 |
|
160 |
160 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
161 |
|
-- Provides **comprehensive data on human genetic diversity**, useful for **evolutionary studies**. |
162 |
|
-- Supports research on **Neanderthal and Denisovan introgression** in modern human populations. |
163 |
|
-- Enhances understanding of **genetic adaptation and disease susceptibility across groups**. |
|
99 |
+- [Explanation of how this study contributes to your subproject goals.] |
|
100 |
+- [Any key arguments or findings that support or challenge your views.] |
164 |
164 |
|
165 |
165 |
--- |
166 |
166 |
|
167 |
167 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
168 |
|
-1. Investigate **functional consequences of genetic variation in underrepresented populations**. |
169 |
|
-2. Study **how selection pressures shaped genetic diversity across different environments**. |
170 |
|
-3. Explore **medical applications of population-specific genetic markers**. |
|
105 |
+1. [Research questions or areas to investigate further.] |
|
106 |
+2. [Potential studies or sources to complement this analysis.] |
171 |
171 |
|
172 |
172 |
--- |
173 |
173 |
|
174 |
174 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
175 |
|
-This study presents **high-coverage genome sequences from 300 individuals across 142 populations**, offering **new insights into global genetic diversity and human evolution**. The findings highlight **deep African population splits, widespread archaic ancestry in non-Africans, and unique variants absent from the human reference genome**. The research enhances our understanding of **migration patterns, adaptation, and evolutionary history**. |
|
111 |
+This study examines **[core research question or focus]**, providing insights into **[main subject area]**. The research utilized **[sample size and methodology]** to assess **[key variables or measured outcomes]**. |
176 |
176 |
|
177 |
|
-This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
|
113 |
+This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study's contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
178 |
178 |
|
179 |
179 |
--- |
180 |
180 |
|
181 |
181 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
182 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nature18964.pdf]] |
|
118 |
+{{velocity}} |
|
119 |
+#set($doi = "[Insert DOI Here]") |
|
120 |
+#set($filename = "${doi}.pdf") |
|
121 |
+#if($xwiki.exists("attach:$filename")) |
|
122 |
+[[Download>>attach:$filename]] |
|
123 |
+#else |
|
124 |
+{{html}}<span style="color: red; font-weight: bold;">🚨 PDF Not Available 🚨</span>{{/html}} |
|
125 |
+#end |
|
126 |
+{{/velocity}} |
183 |
183 |
|
184 |
184 |
{{/expand}} |
185 |
185 |
|
|
130 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
186 |
186 |
|
187 |
|
-== Study: Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies == |
188 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies" expanded="false"}} |
189 |
|
-**Source:** *Nature Genetics* |
190 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2015* |
191 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Tinca J. C. Polderman, Beben Benyamin, Christiaan A. de Leeuw, Patrick F. Sullivan, Arjen van Bochoven, Peter M. Visscher, Danielle Posthuma* |
192 |
|
-**Title:** *"Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies"* |
193 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1038/ng.328](https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.328) |
194 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Genetics, Heritability, Twin Studies, Behavioral Science* |
195 |
195 |
|
196 |
|
---- |
197 |
197 |
|
198 |
|
-## **Key Statistics** |
199 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
200 |
|
- - Analyzed **17,804 traits from 2,748 twin studies** published between **1958 and 2012**. |
201 |
|
- - Included data from **14,558,903 twin pairs**, making it the largest meta-analysis on human heritability. |
202 |
|
- |
203 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
204 |
|
- - Found **49% average heritability** across all traits. |
205 |
|
- - **69% of traits follow a simple additive genetic model**, meaning most variance is due to genes, not environment. |
206 |
|
- |
207 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
208 |
|
- - **Neurological, metabolic, and psychiatric traits** showed the highest heritability estimates. |
209 |
|
- - Traits related to **social values and environmental interactions** had lower heritability estimates. |
210 |
|
- |
211 |
211 |
--- |
212 |
212 |
|
213 |
|
-## **Findings** |
214 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
215 |
|
- - Across all traits, genetic factors play a significant role in individual differences. |
216 |
|
- - The study contradicts models that **overestimate environmental effects in behavioral and cognitive traits**. |
|
136 |
+{{expand title="Study: Trends in Frequency of Sexual Activity and Number of Sexual Partners Among Adults Aged 18 to 44 Years in the US, 2000-2018" expanded="false"}} |
|
137 |
+**Source:** *JAMA Network Open* |
|
138 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2020* |
|
139 |
+**Author(s):** *Ueda P, Mercer CH, Ghaznavi C, Herbenick D.* |
|
140 |
+**Title:** *"Trends in Frequency of Sexual Activity and Number of Sexual Partners Among Adults Aged 18 to 44 Years in the US, 2000-2018"* |
|
141 |
+**DOI:** [10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3833](https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3833) |
|
142 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Public Health, Sexual Behavior, Demography* |
217 |
217 |
|
218 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
219 |
|
- - **Eye and brain-related traits showed the highest heritability (~70-80%)**. |
220 |
|
- - **Shared environmental effects were negligible (<10%) for most traits**. |
221 |
|
- |
222 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
223 |
|
- - Twin correlations suggest **limited evidence for strong non-additive genetic influences**. |
224 |
|
- - The study highlights **missing heritability in complex traits**, which genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have yet to fully explain. |
225 |
|
- |
226 |
226 |
--- |
227 |
227 |
|
228 |
|
-## **Critique and Observations** |
229 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
230 |
|
- - **Largest-ever heritability meta-analysis**, covering nearly all published twin studies. |
231 |
|
- - Provides a **comprehensive framework for understanding gene-environment contributions**. |
232 |
|
- |
233 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
234 |
|
- - **Underrepresentation of African, South American, and Asian twin cohorts**, limiting global generalizability. |
235 |
|
- - Cannot **fully separate genetic influences from potential cultural/environmental confounders**. |
236 |
|
- |
237 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
238 |
|
- - Future research should use **whole-genome sequencing** for finer-grained heritability estimates. |
239 |
|
- - **Incorporate non-Western populations** to assess global heritability trends. |
240 |
|
- |
241 |
|
---- |
242 |
|
- |
243 |
|
-## **Relevance to Subproject** |
244 |
|
-- Establishes a **quantitative benchmark for heritability across human traits**. |
245 |
|
-- Reinforces **genetic influence on cognitive, behavioral, and physical traits**. |
246 |
|
-- Highlights the need for **genome-wide studies to identify missing heritability**. |
247 |
|
- |
248 |
|
---- |
249 |
|
- |
250 |
|
-## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
251 |
|
-1. Investigate how **heritability estimates compare across different socioeconomic backgrounds**. |
252 |
|
-2. Examine **gene-environment interactions in cognitive and psychiatric traits**. |
253 |
|
-3. Explore **non-additive genetic effects on human traits using newer statistical models**. |
254 |
|
- |
255 |
|
---- |
256 |
|
- |
257 |
|
-## **Summary of Research Study** |
258 |
|
-This study presents a **comprehensive meta-analysis of human trait heritability**, covering **over 50 years of twin research**. The findings confirm **genes play a predominant role in shaping human traits**, with an **average heritability of 49%** across all measured characteristics. The research offers **valuable insights into genetic and environmental influences**, guiding future gene-mapping efforts and behavioral genetics studies. |
259 |
|
- |
260 |
|
-This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
261 |
|
- |
262 |
|
---- |
263 |
|
- |
264 |
|
-## **📄 Download Full Study** |
265 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_ng.328.pdf]] |
266 |
|
- |
267 |
|
-{{/expand}} |
268 |
|
- |
269 |
|
- |
270 |
|
-== Study: Genetic Analysis of African Populations: Human Evolution and Complex Disease == |
271 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Genetic Analysis of African Populations: Human Evolution and Complex Disease" expanded="false"}} |
272 |
|
-**Source:** *Nature Reviews Genetics* |
273 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2002* |
274 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Sarah A. Tishkoff, Scott M. Williams* |
275 |
|
-**Title:** *"Genetic Analysis of African Populations: Human Evolution and Complex Disease"* |
276 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1038/nrg865](https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg865) |
277 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Population Genetics, Human Evolution, Complex Diseases* |
278 |
|
- |
279 |
|
---- |
280 |
|
- |
281 |
281 |
## **Key Statistics** |
282 |
282 |
1. **General Observations:** |
283 |
|
- - Africa harbors **the highest genetic diversity** of any region, making it key to understanding human evolution. |
284 |
|
- - The study analyzes **genetic variation and linkage disequilibrium (LD) in African populations**. |
|
148 |
+ - Study analyzed **General Social Survey (2000-2018)** data. |
|
149 |
+ - Found **declining trends in sexual activity** among young adults. |
285 |
285 |
|
286 |
286 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
287 |
|
- - African populations exhibit **greater genetic differentiation compared to non-Africans**. |
288 |
|
- - **Migration and admixture** have shaped modern African genomes over the past **100,000 years**. |
|
152 |
+ - Decreases in sexual activity were most prominent among **men aged 18-34**. |
|
153 |
+ - Factors like **marital status, employment, and psychological well-being** were associated with changes in sexual frequency. |
289 |
289 |
|
290 |
290 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
291 |
|
- - The **effective population size (Ne) of Africans** is higher than that of non-African populations. |
292 |
|
- - LD blocks are **shorter in African genomes**, suggesting more historical recombination events. |
|
156 |
+ - Frequency of sexual activity decreased by **8-10%** over the studied period. |
|
157 |
+ - Number of sexual partners remained **relatively stable** despite declining activity rates. |
293 |
293 |
|
294 |
294 |
--- |
295 |
295 |
|
296 |
296 |
## **Findings** |
297 |
297 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
298 |
|
- - African populations are the **most genetically diverse**, supporting the *Recent African Origin* hypothesis. |
299 |
|
- - Genetic variation in African populations can **help fine-map complex disease genes**. |
|
163 |
+ - A significant decline in sexual frequency, especially among **younger men**. |
|
164 |
+ - Shifts in relationship dynamics and economic stressors may contribute to the trend. |
300 |
300 |
|
301 |
301 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
302 |
|
- - **West Africans exhibit higher genetic diversity** than East Africans due to differing migration patterns. |
303 |
|
- - Populations such as **San hunter-gatherers show deep genetic divergence**. |
|
167 |
+ - More pronounced decline among **unmarried individuals**. |
|
168 |
+ - No major change observed for **married adults** over time. |
304 |
304 |
|
305 |
305 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
306 |
|
- - Admixture in African Americans includes **West African and European genetic contributions**. |
307 |
|
- - SNP (single nucleotide polymorphism) diversity in African genomes **exceeds that of non-African groups**. |
|
171 |
+ - **Mental health and employment status** were correlated with decreased activity. |
|
172 |
+ - Social factors such as **screen time and digital entertainment consumption** are potential contributors. |
308 |
308 |
|
309 |
309 |
--- |
310 |
310 |
|
311 |
311 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
312 |
312 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
313 |
|
- - Provides **comprehensive genetic analysis** of diverse African populations. |
314 |
|
- - Highlights **how genetic diversity impacts health disparities and disease risks**. |
|
178 |
+ - **Large sample size** from a nationally representative dataset. |
|
179 |
+ - **Longitudinal design** enables trend analysis over time. |
315 |
315 |
|
316 |
316 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
317 |
|
- - Many **African populations remain understudied**, limiting full understanding of diversity. |
318 |
|
- - Focuses more on genetic variation than on **specific disease mechanisms**. |
|
182 |
+ - Self-reported data may introduce **response bias**. |
|
183 |
+ - No direct causal mechanisms tested for the decline in sexual activity. |
319 |
319 |
|
320 |
320 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
321 |
|
- - Expand research into **underrepresented African populations**. |
322 |
|
- - Integrate **whole-genome sequencing for a more detailed evolutionary timeline**. |
|
186 |
+ - Further studies should incorporate **qualitative data** on behavioral shifts. |
|
187 |
+ - Additional factors such as **economic shifts and social media usage** need exploration. |
323 |
323 |
|
324 |
324 |
--- |
325 |
325 |
|
326 |
326 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
327 |
|
-- Supports **genetic models of human evolution** and the **out-of-Africa hypothesis**. |
328 |
|
-- Reinforces **Africa’s key role in disease gene mapping and precision medicine**. |
329 |
|
-- Provides insight into **historical migration patterns and their genetic impact**. |
|
192 |
+- Provides evidence on **changing demographic behaviors** in relation to relationships and social interactions. |
|
193 |
+- Highlights the role of **mental health, employment, and societal changes** in personal behaviors. |
330 |
330 |
|
331 |
331 |
--- |
332 |
332 |
|
333 |
333 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
334 |
|
-1. Investigate **genetic adaptations to local environments within Africa**. |
335 |
|
-2. Study **the role of African genetic diversity in disease resistance**. |
336 |
|
-3. Expand research on **how ancient migration patterns shaped modern genetic structure**. |
|
198 |
+1. Investigate the **impact of digital media consumption** on relationship dynamics. |
|
199 |
+2. Examine **regional and cultural differences** in sexual activity trends. |
337 |
337 |
|
338 |
338 |
--- |
339 |
339 |
|
340 |
340 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
341 |
|
-This study explores the **genetic diversity of African populations**, analyzing their role in **human evolution and complex disease research**. The findings highlight **Africa’s unique genetic landscape**, confirming it as the most genetically diverse continent. The research provides valuable insights into **how genetic variation influences disease susceptibility, evolution, and population structure**. |
|
204 |
+This study examines **trends in sexual frequency and number of partners among U.S. adults (2000-2018)**, highlighting significant **declines in sexual activity, particularly among young men**. The research utilized **General Social Survey data** to analyze the impact of **sociodemographic factors, employment status, and mental well-being** on sexual behavior. |
342 |
342 |
|
343 |
|
-This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
|
206 |
+This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study's contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
344 |
344 |
|
345 |
345 |
--- |
346 |
346 |
|
347 |
347 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
348 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nrg865MODERN.pdf]] |
|
211 |
+{{velocity}} |
|
212 |
+#set($doi = "10.1001_jamanetworkopen.2020.3833") |
|
213 |
+#set($filename = "${doi}.pdf") |
|
214 |
+#if($xwiki.exists("attach:$filename")) |
|
215 |
+[[Download>>attach:$filename]] |
|
216 |
+#else |
|
217 |
+{{html}}<span style="color: red; font-weight: bold;">🚨 PDF Not Available 🚨</span>{{/html}} |
|
218 |
+#end |
|
219 |
+{{/velocity}} |
349 |
349 |
|
350 |
350 |
{{/expand}} |
351 |
351 |
|
|
223 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
352 |
352 |
|
353 |
|
-== Study: Pervasive Findings of Directional Selection in Ancient DNA == |
354 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Pervasive Findings of Directional Selection in Ancient DNA" expanded="false"}} |
355 |
|
-**Source:** *bioRxiv Preprint* |
356 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *September 15, 2024* |
357 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Ali Akbari, Alison R. Barton, Steven Gazal, Zheng Li, Mohammadreza Kariminejad, et al.* |
358 |
|
-**Title:** *"Pervasive findings of directional selection realize the promise of ancient DNA to elucidate human adaptation"* |
359 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1101/2024.09.14.613021](https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.09.14.613021) |
360 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Genomics, Evolutionary Biology, Natural Selection* |
361 |
361 |
|
362 |
|
---- |
|
226 |
+{{expand title="Study: One is the Loneliest Number: Involuntary Celibacy (Incel), Mental Health, and Loneliness" expanded="false"}} |
|
227 |
+**Source:** *Current Psychology* |
|
228 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2024* |
|
229 |
+**Author(s):** *Brandon Sparks, Alexandra M. Zidenberg, Mark E. Olver* |
|
230 |
+**Title:** *"One is the Loneliest Number: Involuntary Celibacy (Incel), Mental Health, and Loneliness"* |
|
231 |
+**DOI:** [10.1007/s12144-023-04275-z](https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04275-z) |
|
232 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Psychology, Mental Health, Social Isolation* |
363 |
363 |
|
364 |
|
-## **Key Statistics** |
365 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
366 |
|
- - Study analyzes **8,433 ancient individuals** from the past **14,000 years**. |
367 |
|
- - Identifies **347 genome-wide significant loci** showing strong selection. |
368 |
|
- |
369 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
370 |
|
- - Examines **West Eurasian populations** and their genetic evolution. |
371 |
|
- - Tracks **changes in allele frequencies over millennia**. |
372 |
|
- |
373 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
374 |
|
- - **10,000 years of directional selection** affected metabolic, immune, and cognitive traits. |
375 |
|
- - **Strong selection signals** found for traits like **skin pigmentation, cognitive function, and immunity**. |
376 |
|
- |
377 |
377 |
--- |
378 |
378 |
|
379 |
|
-## **Findings** |
380 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
381 |
|
- - **Hundreds of alleles have been subject to directional selection** over recent millennia. |
382 |
|
- - Traits like **immune function, metabolism, and cognitive performance** show strong selection. |
383 |
|
- |
384 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
385 |
|
- - Selection pressure on **energy storage genes** supports the **Thrifty Gene Hypothesis**. |
386 |
|
- - **Cognitive performance-related alleles** have undergone selection, but their historical advantages remain unclear. |
387 |
|
- |
388 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
389 |
|
- - **Celiac disease risk allele** increased from **0% to 20%** in 4,000 years. |
390 |
|
- - **Blood type B frequency rose from 0% to 8% in 6,000 years**. |
391 |
|
- - **Tuberculosis risk allele** fluctuated from **2% to 9% over 3,000 years before declining**. |
392 |
|
- |
393 |
|
---- |
394 |
|
- |
395 |
|
-## **Critique and Observations** |
396 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
397 |
|
- - **Largest dataset to date** on natural selection in human ancient DNA. |
398 |
|
- - Uses **direct allele frequency tracking instead of indirect measures**. |
399 |
|
- |
400 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
401 |
|
- - Findings **may not translate directly** to modern populations. |
402 |
|
- - **Unclear whether observed selection pressures persist today**. |
403 |
|
- |
404 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
405 |
|
- - Expanding research to **other global populations** to assess universal trends. |
406 |
|
- - Investigating **long-term evolutionary trade-offs of selected alleles**. |
407 |
|
- |
408 |
|
---- |
409 |
|
- |
410 |
|
-## **Relevance to Subproject** |
411 |
|
-- Provides **direct evidence of long-term genetic adaptation** in human populations. |
412 |
|
-- Supports theories on **polygenic selection shaping human cognition, metabolism, and immunity**. |
413 |
|
-- Highlights **how past selection pressures may still influence modern health and disease prevalence**. |
414 |
|
- |
415 |
|
---- |
416 |
|
- |
417 |
|
-## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
418 |
|
-1. Examine **selection patterns in non-European populations** for comparison. |
419 |
|
-2. Investigate **how environmental and cultural shifts influenced genetic selection**. |
420 |
|
-3. Explore **the genetic basis of traits linked to past and present-day human survival**. |
421 |
|
- |
422 |
|
---- |
423 |
|
- |
424 |
|
-## **Summary of Research Study** |
425 |
|
-This study examines **how human genetic adaptation has unfolded over 14,000 years**, using a **large dataset of ancient DNA**. It highlights **strong selection on immune function, metabolism, and cognitive traits**, revealing **hundreds of loci affected by directional selection**. The findings emphasize **the power of ancient DNA in tracking human evolution and adaptation**. |
426 |
|
- |
427 |
|
---- |
428 |
|
- |
429 |
|
-## **📄 Download Full Study** |
430 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1101_2024.09.14.613021doi_.pdf]] |
431 |
|
- |
432 |
|
-{{/expand}} |
433 |
|
- |
434 |
|
-== Study: The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age == |
435 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age" expanded="false"}} |
436 |
|
-**Source:** *Twin Research and Human Genetics (Cambridge University Press)* |
437 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2013* |
438 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Thomas J. Bouchard Jr.* |
439 |
|
-**Title:** *"The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age"* |
440 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1017/thg.2013.54](https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2013.54) |
441 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Intelligence, Heritability, Developmental Psychology* |
442 |
|
- |
443 |
|
---- |
444 |
|
- |
445 |
445 |
## **Key Statistics** |
446 |
446 |
1. **General Observations:** |
447 |
|
- - The study documents how the **heritability of IQ increases with age**, reaching an asymptote at **0.80 by adulthood**. |
448 |
|
- - Analysis is based on **longitudinal twin and adoption studies**. |
|
238 |
+ - Study analyzed **67 self-identified incels** and **103 non-incel men**. |
|
239 |
+ - Incels reported **higher loneliness and lower social support** compared to non-incels. |
449 |
449 |
|
450 |
450 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
451 |
|
- - Shared environmental influence on IQ **declines with age**, reaching **0.10 in adulthood**. |
452 |
|
- - Monozygotic twins show **increasing genetic similarity in IQ over time**, while dizygotic twins become **less concordant**. |
|
242 |
+ - Incels exhibited **higher levels of depression, anxiety, and self-critical rumination**. |
|
243 |
+ - **Social isolation was a key factor** differentiating incels from non-incels. |
453 |
453 |
|
454 |
454 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
455 |
|
- - Data from the **Louisville Longitudinal Twin Study and cross-national twin samples** support findings. |
456 |
|
- - IQ stability over time is **influenced more by genetics than by shared environmental factors**. |
|
246 |
+ - 95% of incels in the study reported **having depression**, with 38% receiving a formal diagnosis. |
|
247 |
+ - **Higher externalization of blame** was linked to stronger incel identification. |
457 |
457 |
|
458 |
458 |
--- |
459 |
459 |
|
460 |
460 |
## **Findings** |
461 |
461 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
462 |
|
- - Intelligence heritability **strengthens throughout development**, contrary to early environmental models. |
463 |
|
- - Shared environmental effects **decrease by late adolescence**, emphasizing **genetic influence in adulthood**. |
|
253 |
+ - Incels experience **heightened rejection sensitivity and loneliness**. |
|
254 |
+ - Lack of social support correlates with **worse mental health outcomes**. |
464 |
464 |
|
465 |
465 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
466 |
|
- - Studies from **Scotland, Netherlands, and the US** show **consistent patterns of increasing heritability with age**. |
467 |
|
- - Findings hold across **varied socio-economic and educational backgrounds**. |
|
257 |
+ - **Avoidant attachment styles** were a strong predictor of incel identity. |
|
258 |
+ - **Mate value perceptions** significantly differed between incels and non-incels. |
468 |
468 |
|
469 |
469 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
470 |
|
- - Longitudinal adoption studies show **declining impact of adoptive parental influence on IQ** as children age. |
471 |
|
- - Cross-sectional twin data confirm **higher IQ correlations for monozygotic twins in adulthood**. |
|
261 |
+ - Incels **engaged in fewer positive coping mechanisms** such as emotional support or positive reframing. |
|
262 |
+ - Instead, they relied on **solitary coping strategies**, worsening their isolation. |
472 |
472 |
|
473 |
473 |
--- |
474 |
474 |
|
475 |
475 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
476 |
476 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
477 |
|
- - **Robust dataset covering multiple twin and adoption studies over decades**. |
478 |
|
- - **Clear, replicable trend** demonstrating the increasing role of genetics in intelligence. |
|
268 |
+ - **First quantitative study** on incels’ social isolation and mental health. |
|
269 |
+ - **Robust sample size** and validated psychological measures. |
479 |
479 |
|
480 |
480 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
481 |
|
- - Findings apply primarily to **Western industrialized nations**, limiting generalizability. |
482 |
|
- - **Lack of neurobiological mechanisms** explaining how genes express their influence over time. |
|
272 |
+ - Sample drawn from **Reddit communities**, which may not represent all incels. |
|
273 |
+ - **No causal conclusions**—correlations between isolation and inceldom need further research. |
483 |
483 |
|
484 |
484 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
485 |
|
- - Future research should investigate **gene-environment interactions in cognitive aging**. |
486 |
|
- - Examine **heritability trends in non-Western populations** to determine cross-cultural consistency. |
|
276 |
+ - Future studies should **compare incel forum users vs. non-users**. |
|
277 |
+ - Investigate **potential intervention strategies** for social integration. |
487 |
487 |
|
488 |
488 |
--- |
489 |
489 |
|
490 |
490 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
491 |
|
-- Provides **strong evidence for the genetic basis of intelligence**. |
492 |
|
-- Highlights the **diminishing role of shared environment in cognitive development**. |
493 |
|
-- Supports research on **cognitive aging and heritability across the lifespan**. |
|
282 |
+- Highlights **mental health vulnerabilities** within the incel community. |
|
283 |
+- Supports research on **loneliness, attachment styles, and social dominance orientation**. |
|
284 |
+- Examines how **peer rejection influences self-perceived mate value**. |
494 |
494 |
|
495 |
495 |
--- |
496 |
496 |
|
497 |
497 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
498 |
|
-1. Investigate **neurogenetic pathways underlying IQ development**. |
499 |
|
-2. Examine **how education and socioeconomic factors interact with genetic IQ influences**. |
500 |
|
-3. Study **heritability trends in aging populations and cognitive decline**. |
|
289 |
+1. Explore how **online community participation** affects incel mental health. |
|
290 |
+2. Investigate **cognitive biases** influencing self-perceived rejection among incels. |
|
291 |
+3. Assess **therapeutic interventions** to address incel social isolation. |
501 |
501 |
|
502 |
502 |
--- |
503 |
503 |
|
504 |
504 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
505 |
|
-This study documents **The Wilson Effect**, demonstrating how the **heritability of IQ increases throughout development**, reaching a plateau of **0.80 by adulthood**. The findings indicate that **shared environmental effects diminish with age**, while **genetic influences on intelligence strengthen**. Using **longitudinal twin and adoption data**, the research provides **strong empirical support for the increasing role of genetics in cognitive ability over time**. |
|
296 |
+This study examines the **psychological characteristics of self-identified incels**, comparing them with non-incel men in terms of **mental health, loneliness, and coping strategies**. The research found **higher depression, anxiety, and avoidant attachment styles among incels**, as well as **greater reliance on solitary coping mechanisms**. It suggests that **lack of social support plays a critical role in exacerbating incel identity and related mental health concerns**. |
506 |
506 |
|
507 |
507 |
This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
508 |
508 |
|
... |
... |
@@ -509,165 +509,149 @@ |
509 |
509 |
--- |
510 |
510 |
|
511 |
511 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
512 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1017_thg.2013.54.pdf]] |
|
303 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1007_s12144-023-04275-z.pdf]] |
513 |
513 |
|
514 |
514 |
{{/expand}} |
515 |
515 |
|
516 |
|
-== Study: Is Homo sapiens polytypic? Human taxonomic diversity and its implications == |
517 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Is Homo sapiens polytypic? Human taxonomic diversity and its implications" expanded="false"}} |
518 |
|
-**Source:** *Medical Hypotheses (Elsevier)* |
519 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2010* |
520 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Michael A. Woodley* |
521 |
|
-**Title:** *"Is Homo sapiens polytypic? Human taxonomic diversity and its implications"* |
522 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1016/j.mehy.2009.07.046](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2009.07.046) |
523 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Human Taxonomy, Evolutionary Biology, Anthropology* |
|
307 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
524 |
524 |
|
525 |
|
---- |
|
309 |
+{{expand title="Study: Associations Between Cannabis Use and Mental Health Symptoms in Young Adults" expanded="false"}} Source: Addictive Behaviors |
|
310 |
+Date of Publication: 2016 |
|
311 |
+Author(s): Andrea Hussong, Christy Capron, Gregory T. Smith, Jennifer L. Maggs |
|
312 |
+Title: "Associations Between Cannabis Use and Mental Health Symptoms in Young Adults" |
|
313 |
+DOI: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.02.030 |
|
314 |
+Subject Matter: Substance Use, Mental Health, Adolescent Development |
526 |
526 |
|
527 |
|
-## **Key Statistics** |
528 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
529 |
|
- - The study argues that **Homo sapiens is polytypic**, meaning it consists of multiple subspecies rather than a single monotypic species. |
530 |
|
- - Examines **genetic diversity, morphological variation, and evolutionary lineage** in humans. |
|
316 |
+Key Statistics |
|
317 |
+General Observations: |
531 |
531 |
|
532 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
533 |
|
- - Discusses **four primary definitions of race/subspecies**: Essentialist, Taxonomic, Population-based, and Lineage-based. |
534 |
|
- - Suggests that **human heterozygosity levels are comparable to species that are classified as polytypic**. |
|
319 |
+Study examined cannabis use trends in young adults over time. |
|
320 |
+Found significant correlations between cannabis use and increased depressive symptoms. |
|
321 |
+Subgroup Analysis: |
535 |
535 |
|
536 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
537 |
|
- - The study evaluates **FST values (genetic differentiation measure)** and argues that human genetic differentiation is comparable to that of recognized subspecies in other species. |
538 |
|
- - Considers **phylogenetic species concepts** in defining human variation. |
|
323 |
+Males exhibited higher rates of cannabis use, but females reported stronger mental health impacts. |
|
324 |
+Individuals with pre-existing anxiety disorders were more likely to report problematic cannabis use. |
|
325 |
+Other Significant Data Points: |
539 |
539 |
|
540 |
|
---- |
|
327 |
+Frequent cannabis users showed a 23% higher likelihood of developing anxiety symptoms. |
|
328 |
+Co-occurring substance use (e.g., alcohol) exacerbated negative psychological effects. |
|
329 |
+Findings |
|
330 |
+Primary Observations: |
541 |
541 |
|
542 |
|
-## **Findings** |
543 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
544 |
|
- - Proposes that **modern human populations meet biological criteria for subspecies classification**. |
545 |
|
- - Highlights **medical and evolutionary implications** of human taxonomic diversity. |
|
332 |
+Cannabis use was linked to higher depressive and anxiety symptoms, particularly in frequent users. |
|
333 |
+Self-medication patterns emerged among those with pre-existing mental health conditions. |
|
334 |
+Subgroup Trends: |
546 |
546 |
|
547 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
548 |
|
- - Discusses **how race concepts evolved over time** in biological sciences. |
549 |
|
- - Compares **human diversity with that of other primates** such as chimpanzees and gorillas. |
|
336 |
+Early cannabis initiation (before age 16) was associated with greater mental health risks. |
|
337 |
+College-aged users reported more impairments in daily functioning due to cannabis use. |
|
338 |
+Specific Case Analysis: |
550 |
550 |
|
551 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
552 |
|
- - Evaluates how **genetic markers correlate with population structure**. |
553 |
|
- - Addresses the **controversy over race classification in modern anthropology**. |
|
340 |
+Participants with a history of childhood trauma were twice as likely to develop problematic cannabis use. |
|
341 |
+Co-use of cannabis and alcohol significantly increased impulsivity scores in the study sample. |
|
342 |
+Critique and Observations |
|
343 |
+Strengths of the Study: |
554 |
554 |
|
555 |
|
---- |
|
345 |
+Large, longitudinal dataset with a diverse sample of young adults. |
|
346 |
+Controlled for confounding variables like socioeconomic status and prior substance use. |
|
347 |
+Limitations of the Study: |
556 |
556 |
|
557 |
|
-## **Critique and Observations** |
558 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
559 |
|
- - Uses **comparative species analysis** to assess human classification. |
560 |
|
- - Provides a **biological perspective** on the race concept, moving beyond social constructivism arguments. |
|
349 |
+Self-reported cannabis use may introduce bias in reported frequency and effects. |
|
350 |
+Did not assess specific THC potency levels, which could influence mental health outcomes. |
|
351 |
+Suggestions for Improvement: |
561 |
561 |
|
562 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
563 |
|
- - Controversial topic with **strong opposing views in anthropology and genetics**. |
564 |
|
- - **Relies on broad genetic trends**, but does not analyze individual-level genetic variation in depth. |
|
353 |
+Future research should investigate dose-dependent effects of cannabis on mental health. |
|
354 |
+Assess long-term psychological outcomes of early cannabis exposure. |
|
355 |
+Relevance to Subproject |
|
356 |
+Supports mental health risk assessment models related to substance use. |
|
357 |
+Highlights gender differences in substance-related psychological impacts. |
|
358 |
+Provides insight into self-medication behaviors among young adults. |
|
359 |
+Suggestions for Further Exploration |
|
360 |
+Investigate the long-term impact of cannabis use on neurodevelopment. |
|
361 |
+Examine the role of genetic predisposition in cannabis-related mental health risks. |
|
362 |
+Assess regional differences in cannabis use trends post-legalization. |
|
363 |
+Summary of Research Study |
|
364 |
+This study examines the relationship between cannabis use and mental health symptoms in young adults, focusing on depressive and anxiety-related outcomes. Using a longitudinal dataset, the researchers found higher risks of anxiety and depression in frequent cannabis users, particularly among those with pre-existing mental health conditions or early cannabis initiation. |
565 |
565 |
|
566 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
567 |
|
- - Further research should **incorporate whole-genome studies** to refine subspecies classifications. |
568 |
|
- - Investigate **how admixture affects taxonomic classification over time**. |
569 |
|
- |
570 |
|
---- |
571 |
|
- |
572 |
|
-## **Relevance to Subproject** |
573 |
|
-- Contributes to discussions on **evolutionary taxonomy and species classification**. |
574 |
|
-- Provides evidence on **genetic differentiation among human populations**. |
575 |
|
-- Highlights **historical and contemporary scientific debates on race and human variation**. |
576 |
|
- |
577 |
|
---- |
578 |
|
- |
579 |
|
-## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
580 |
|
-1. Examine **FST values in modern and ancient human populations**. |
581 |
|
-2. Investigate how **adaptive evolution influences population differentiation**. |
582 |
|
-3. Explore **the impact of genetic diversity on medical treatments and disease susceptibility**. |
583 |
|
- |
584 |
|
---- |
585 |
|
- |
586 |
|
-## **Summary of Research Study** |
587 |
|
-This study evaluates **whether Homo sapiens should be classified as a polytypic species**, analyzing **genetic diversity, evolutionary lineage, and morphological variation**. Using comparative analysis with other primates and mammals, the research suggests that **human populations meet biological criteria for subspecies classification**, with implications for **evolutionary biology, anthropology, and medicine**. |
588 |
|
- |
589 |
589 |
This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
590 |
590 |
|
591 |
|
---- |
|
368 |
+📄 Download Full Study |
|
369 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.addbeh.2016.02.030.pdf]] |
592 |
592 |
|
593 |
|
-## **📄 Download Full Study** |
594 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.mehy.2009.07.046.pdf]] |
595 |
|
- |
596 |
596 |
{{/expand}} |
597 |
597 |
|
|
373 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
598 |
598 |
|
599 |
|
-== Study: Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence: Intelligence Research, Experts' Background, Controversial Issues, and the Media == |
600 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence: Intelligence Research, Experts' Background, Controversial Issues, and the Media" expanded="false"}} |
|
375 |
+{{expand title="Study: Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time?" expanded="false"}} |
601 |
601 |
**Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)* |
602 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2019* |
603 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Heiner Rindermann, David Becker, Thomas R. Coyle* |
604 |
|
-**Title:** *"Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence: Intelligence Research, Experts' Background, Controversial Issues, and the Media"* |
605 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2019.101406](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2019.101406) |
606 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Psychology, Intelligence Research, Expert Analysis* |
|
377 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2014* |
|
378 |
+**Author(s):** *Michael A. Woodley, Jan te Nijenhuis, Raegan Murphy* |
|
379 |
+**Title:** *"Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time?"* |
|
380 |
+**DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.012](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.012) |
|
381 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Cognitive Decline, Intelligence, Dysgenics* |
607 |
607 |
|
608 |
608 |
--- |
609 |
609 |
|
610 |
610 |
## **Key Statistics** |
611 |
611 |
1. **General Observations:** |
612 |
|
- - Survey of **102 experts** on intelligence research and public discourse. |
613 |
|
- - Evaluated experts' backgrounds, political affiliations, and views on controversial topics in intelligence research. |
|
387 |
+ - The study examines reaction time data from **13 age-matched studies** spanning **1884–2004**. |
|
388 |
+ - Results suggest an estimated **decline of 13.35 IQ points** over this period. |
614 |
614 |
|
615 |
615 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
616 |
|
- - **90% of experts were from Western countries**, and **83% were male**. |
617 |
|
- - Political spectrum ranged from **54% left-liberal, 24% conservative**, with significant ideological influences on views. |
|
391 |
+ - The study found **slower reaction times in modern populations** compared to Victorian-era individuals. |
|
392 |
+ - Data from **Western countries (US, UK, Canada, Australia, Finland)** were analyzed. |
618 |
618 |
|
619 |
619 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
620 |
|
- - Experts rated media coverage of intelligence research as **poor (avg. 3.1 on a 9-point scale)**. |
621 |
|
- - **50% of experts attributed US Black-White IQ differences to genetic factors, 50% to environmental factors**. |
|
395 |
+ - The estimated **dysgenic rate is 1.21 IQ points lost per decade**. |
|
396 |
+ - Meta-regression analysis confirmed a **steady secular trend in slowing reaction time**. |
622 |
622 |
|
623 |
623 |
--- |
624 |
624 |
|
625 |
625 |
## **Findings** |
626 |
626 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
627 |
|
- - Experts overwhelmingly support **the g-factor theory of intelligence**. |
628 |
|
- - **Heritability of intelligence** was widely accepted, though views differed on race and group differences. |
|
402 |
+ - Supports the hypothesis of **intelligence decline due to genetic and environmental factors**. |
|
403 |
+ - Reaction time, a **biomarker for cognitive ability**, has slowed significantly over time. |
629 |
629 |
|
630 |
630 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
631 |
|
- - **Left-leaning experts were more likely to reject genetic explanations for group IQ differences**. |
632 |
|
- - **Right-leaning experts tended to favor a stronger role for genetic factors** in intelligence disparities. |
|
406 |
+ - A stronger **correlation between slower reaction time and lower general intelligence (g)**. |
|
407 |
+ - Flynn effect (IQ gains) does not contradict this finding, as reaction time is a **biological, not environmental, measure**. |
633 |
633 |
|
634 |
634 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
635 |
|
- - The study compared **media coverage of intelligence research** with expert opinions. |
636 |
|
- - Found a **disconnect between journalists and intelligence researchers**, especially regarding politically sensitive issues. |
|
410 |
+ - Cross-national comparisons indicate a **global trend in slower reaction times**. |
|
411 |
+ - Factors like **modern neurotoxin exposure** and **reduced selective pressure for intelligence** may contribute. |
637 |
637 |
|
638 |
638 |
--- |
639 |
639 |
|
640 |
640 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
641 |
641 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
642 |
|
- - **Largest expert survey on intelligence research** to date. |
643 |
|
- - Provides insight into **how political orientation influences scientific perspectives**. |
|
417 |
+ - **Comprehensive meta-analysis** covering over a century of reaction time data. |
|
418 |
+ - **Robust statistical corrections** for measurement variance between historical and modern studies. |
644 |
644 |
|
645 |
645 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
646 |
|
- - **Sample primarily from Western countries**, limiting global perspectives. |
647 |
|
- - Self-selection bias may skew responses toward **those more willing to engage with controversial topics**. |
|
421 |
+ - Some historical data sources **lack methodological consistency**. |
|
422 |
+ - **Reaction time measurements vary by study**, requiring adjustments for equipment differences. |
648 |
648 |
|
649 |
649 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
650 |
|
- - Future studies should include **a broader range of global experts**. |
651 |
|
- - Additional research needed on **media biases and misrepresentation of intelligence research**. |
|
425 |
+ - Future studies should **replicate results with more modern datasets**. |
|
426 |
+ - Investigate **alternative cognitive biomarkers** for intelligence over time. |
652 |
652 |
|
653 |
653 |
--- |
654 |
654 |
|
655 |
655 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
656 |
|
-- Provides insight into **expert consensus and division on intelligence research**. |
657 |
|
-- Highlights the **role of media bias** in shaping public perception of intelligence science. |
658 |
|
-- Useful for understanding **the intersection of science, politics, and public discourse** on intelligence research. |
|
431 |
+- Provides evidence for **long-term intelligence trends**, contributing to research on **cognitive evolution**. |
|
432 |
+- Aligns with broader discussions on **dysgenics, neurophysiology, and cognitive load**. |
|
433 |
+- Supports the argument that **modern societies may be experiencing intelligence decline**. |
659 |
659 |
|
660 |
660 |
--- |
661 |
661 |
|
662 |
662 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
663 |
|
-1. Examine **cross-national differences** in expert opinions on intelligence. |
664 |
|
-2. Investigate how **media bias impacts public understanding of intelligence research**. |
665 |
|
-3. Conduct follow-up studies with **a more diverse expert pool** to test findings. |
|
438 |
+1. Investigate **genetic markers associated with reaction time** and intelligence decline. |
|
439 |
+2. Examine **regional variations in reaction time trends**. |
|
440 |
+3. Explore **cognitive resilience factors that counteract the decline**. |
666 |
666 |
|
667 |
667 |
--- |
668 |
668 |
|
669 |
669 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
670 |
|
-This study surveys **expert opinions on intelligence research**, analyzing **how backgrounds, political ideologies, and media representation influence perspectives on intelligence**. The findings highlight **divisions in scientific consensus**, particularly on **genetic vs. environmental causes of IQ disparities**. Additionally, the research uncovers **widespread dissatisfaction with media portrayals of intelligence research**, pointing to **the impact of ideological biases on public discourse**. |
|
445 |
+This study examines **historical reaction time data** as a measure of **cognitive ability and intelligence decline**, analyzing data from **Western populations between 1884 and 2004**. The results suggest a **measurable decline in intelligence, estimated at 13.35 IQ points**, likely due to **dysgenic fertility, neurophysiological factors, and reduced selection pressures**. |
671 |
671 |
|
672 |
672 |
This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
673 |
673 |
|
... |
... |
@@ -674,12 +674,12 @@ |
674 |
674 |
--- |
675 |
675 |
|
676 |
676 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
677 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2019.101406.pdf]] |
|
452 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2014.05.012.pdf]] |
678 |
678 |
|
679 |
679 |
{{/expand}} |
680 |
680 |
|
|
456 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
681 |
681 |
|
682 |
|
-== Study: A Review of Intelligence GWAS Hits: Their Relationship to Country IQ and the Issue of Spatial Autocorrelation == |
683 |
683 |
{{expand title="Study: A Review of Intelligence GWAS Hits: Their Relationship to Country IQ and the Issue of Spatial Autocorrelation" expanded="false"}} |
684 |
684 |
**Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)* |
685 |
685 |
**Date of Publication:** *2015* |
... |
... |
@@ -761,287 +761,245 @@ |
761 |
761 |
|
762 |
762 |
{{/expand}} |
763 |
763 |
|
764 |
|
-== Study: Genetic Structure, Self-Identified Race/Ethnicity, and Confounding == |
765 |
|
-{{expand expanded="false" title="Click here to expand details"}} |
766 |
|
-**Source:** Journal of Genetic Epidemiology |
767 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** 2024-01-15 |
768 |
|
-**Author(s):** Smith et al. |
769 |
|
-**Title:** "Genetic Structure, Self-Identified Race/Ethnicity, and Confounding in Case-Control Association Studies" |
770 |
|
-**DOI:** [https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235](https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235) |
771 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** Genetics, Social Science |
|
539 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
772 |
772 |
|
773 |
|
-**Tags:** `Genetics` `Race & Ethnicity` `Biomedical Research` |
|
541 |
+{{expand title="Study: Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence: Intelligence Research, Experts' Background, Controversial Issues, and the Media" expanded="false"}} |
|
542 |
+**Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)* |
|
543 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2019* |
|
544 |
+**Author(s):** *Heiner Rindermann, David Becker, Thomas R. Coyle* |
|
545 |
+**Title:** *"Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence: Intelligence Research, Experts' Background, Controversial Issues, and the Media"* |
|
546 |
+**DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2019.101406](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2019.101406) |
|
547 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Psychology, Intelligence Research, Expert Analysis* |
774 |
774 |
|
775 |
|
-=== **Key Statistics** === |
776 |
|
- |
777 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
778 |
|
- - A near-perfect alignment between self-identified race/ethnicity (SIRE) and genetic ancestry was observed. |
779 |
|
- - Misclassification rate: **0.14%**. |
780 |
|
- |
781 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
782 |
|
- - Four groups analyzed: **White, African American, East Asian, and Hispanic**. |
783 |
|
- - Hispanic genetic clusters showed significant European and Native American lineage. |
784 |
|
- |
785 |
|
-=== **Findings** === |
786 |
|
- |
787 |
|
-- Self-identified race strongly aligns with genetic ancestry. |
788 |
|
-- Minor discrepancies exist but do not significantly impact classification. |
789 |
|
- |
790 |
|
-=== **Relevance to Subproject** === |
791 |
|
- |
792 |
|
-- Reinforces the reliability of **self-reported racial identity** in genetic research. |
793 |
|
-- Highlights **policy considerations** in biomedical studies. |
794 |
|
-{{/expand}} |
795 |
|
- |
796 |
|
- |
797 |
797 |
--- |
798 |
798 |
|
799 |
|
-= Dating and Interpersonal Relationships = |
800 |
|
- |
801 |
|
-== Study: Trends in Frequency of Sexual Activity and Number of Sexual Partners Among Adults Aged 18 to 44 Years in the US, 2000-2018 == |
802 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Trends in Frequency of Sexual Activity and Number of Sexual Partners Among Adults Aged 18 to 44 Years in the US, 2000-2018" expanded="false"}} |
803 |
|
-**Source:** *JAMA Network Open* |
804 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2020* |
805 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Ueda P, Mercer CH, Ghaznavi C, Herbenick D.* |
806 |
|
-**Title:** *"Trends in Frequency of Sexual Activity and Number of Sexual Partners Among Adults Aged 18 to 44 Years in the US, 2000-2018"* |
807 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3833](https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3833) |
808 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Public Health, Sexual Behavior, Demography* |
809 |
|
- |
810 |
|
---- |
811 |
|
- |
812 |
812 |
## **Key Statistics** |
813 |
813 |
1. **General Observations:** |
814 |
|
- - Study analyzed **General Social Survey (2000-2018)** data. |
815 |
|
- - Found **declining trends in sexual activity** among young adults. |
|
553 |
+ - Survey of **102 experts** on intelligence research and public discourse. |
|
554 |
+ - Evaluated experts' backgrounds, political affiliations, and views on controversial topics in intelligence research. |
816 |
816 |
|
817 |
817 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
818 |
|
- - Decreases in sexual activity were most prominent among **men aged 18-34**. |
819 |
|
- - Factors like **marital status, employment, and psychological well-being** were associated with changes in sexual frequency. |
|
557 |
+ - **90% of experts were from Western countries**, and **83% were male**. |
|
558 |
+ - Political spectrum ranged from **54% left-liberal, 24% conservative**, with significant ideological influences on views. |
820 |
820 |
|
821 |
821 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
822 |
|
- - Frequency of sexual activity decreased by **8-10%** over the studied period. |
823 |
|
- - Number of sexual partners remained **relatively stable** despite declining activity rates. |
|
561 |
+ - Experts rated media coverage of intelligence research as **poor (avg. 3.1 on a 9-point scale)**. |
|
562 |
+ - **50% of experts attributed US Black-White IQ differences to genetic factors, 50% to environmental factors**. |
824 |
824 |
|
825 |
825 |
--- |
826 |
826 |
|
827 |
827 |
## **Findings** |
828 |
828 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
829 |
|
- - A significant decline in sexual frequency, especially among **younger men**. |
830 |
|
- - Shifts in relationship dynamics and economic stressors may contribute to the trend. |
|
568 |
+ - Experts overwhelmingly support **the g-factor theory of intelligence**. |
|
569 |
+ - **Heritability of intelligence** was widely accepted, though views differed on race and group differences. |
831 |
831 |
|
832 |
832 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
833 |
|
- - More pronounced decline among **unmarried individuals**. |
834 |
|
- - No major change observed for **married adults** over time. |
|
572 |
+ - **Left-leaning experts were more likely to reject genetic explanations for group IQ differences**. |
|
573 |
+ - **Right-leaning experts tended to favor a stronger role for genetic factors** in intelligence disparities. |
835 |
835 |
|
836 |
836 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
837 |
|
- - **Mental health and employment status** were correlated with decreased activity. |
838 |
|
- - Social factors such as **screen time and digital entertainment consumption** are potential contributors. |
|
576 |
+ - The study compared **media coverage of intelligence research** with expert opinions. |
|
577 |
+ - Found a **disconnect between journalists and intelligence researchers**, especially regarding politically sensitive issues. |
839 |
839 |
|
840 |
840 |
--- |
841 |
841 |
|
842 |
842 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
843 |
843 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
844 |
|
- - **Large sample size** from a nationally representative dataset. |
845 |
|
- - **Longitudinal design** enables trend analysis over time. |
|
583 |
+ - **Largest expert survey on intelligence research** to date. |
|
584 |
+ - Provides insight into **how political orientation influences scientific perspectives**. |
846 |
846 |
|
847 |
847 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
848 |
|
- - Self-reported data may introduce **response bias**. |
849 |
|
- - No direct causal mechanisms tested for the decline in sexual activity. |
|
587 |
+ - **Sample primarily from Western countries**, limiting global perspectives. |
|
588 |
+ - Self-selection bias may skew responses toward **those more willing to engage with controversial topics**. |
850 |
850 |
|
851 |
851 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
852 |
|
- - Further studies should incorporate **qualitative data** on behavioral shifts. |
853 |
|
- - Additional factors such as **economic shifts and social media usage** need exploration. |
|
591 |
+ - Future studies should include **a broader range of global experts**. |
|
592 |
+ - Additional research needed on **media biases and misrepresentation of intelligence research**. |
854 |
854 |
|
855 |
855 |
--- |
856 |
856 |
|
857 |
857 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
858 |
|
-- Provides evidence on **changing demographic behaviors** in relation to relationships and social interactions. |
859 |
|
-- Highlights the role of **mental health, employment, and societal changes** in personal behaviors. |
|
597 |
+- Provides insight into **expert consensus and division on intelligence research**. |
|
598 |
+- Highlights the **role of media bias** in shaping public perception of intelligence science. |
|
599 |
+- Useful for understanding **the intersection of science, politics, and public discourse** on intelligence research. |
860 |
860 |
|
861 |
861 |
--- |
862 |
862 |
|
863 |
863 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
864 |
|
-1. Investigate the **impact of digital media consumption** on relationship dynamics. |
865 |
|
-2. Examine **regional and cultural differences** in sexual activity trends. |
|
604 |
+1. Examine **cross-national differences** in expert opinions on intelligence. |
|
605 |
+2. Investigate how **media bias impacts public understanding of intelligence research**. |
|
606 |
+3. Conduct follow-up studies with **a more diverse expert pool** to test findings. |
866 |
866 |
|
867 |
867 |
--- |
868 |
868 |
|
869 |
869 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
870 |
|
-This study examines **trends in sexual frequency and number of partners among U.S. adults (2000-2018)**, highlighting significant **declines in sexual activity, particularly among young men**. The research utilized **General Social Survey data** to analyze the impact of **sociodemographic factors, employment status, and mental well-being** on sexual behavior. |
|
611 |
+This study surveys **expert opinions on intelligence research**, analyzing **how backgrounds, political ideologies, and media representation influence perspectives on intelligence**. The findings highlight **divisions in scientific consensus**, particularly on **genetic vs. environmental causes of IQ disparities**. Additionally, the research uncovers **widespread dissatisfaction with media portrayals of intelligence research**, pointing to **the impact of ideological biases on public discourse**. |
871 |
871 |
|
872 |
|
-This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study's contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
|
613 |
+This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
873 |
873 |
|
874 |
874 |
--- |
875 |
875 |
|
876 |
876 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
877 |
|
-{{velocity}} |
878 |
|
-#set($doi = "10.1001_jamanetworkopen.2020.3833") |
879 |
|
-#set($filename = "${doi}.pdf") |
880 |
|
-#if($xwiki.exists("attach:$filename")) |
881 |
|
-[[Download>>attach:$filename]] |
882 |
|
-#else |
883 |
|
-{{html}}<span style="color: red; font-weight: bold;">🚨 PDF Not Available 🚨</span>{{/html}} |
884 |
|
-#end |
885 |
|
-{{/velocity}} |
|
618 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2019.101406.pdf]] |
886 |
886 |
|
887 |
887 |
{{/expand}} |
888 |
888 |
|
|
622 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
889 |
889 |
|
890 |
|
-== Study: Biracial Couples and Adverse Birth Outcomes – A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis == |
891 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Biracial Couples and Adverse Birth Outcomes – A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" expanded="false"}} |
892 |
|
-**Source:** *Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica* |
893 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2012* |
894 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Ravisha M. Srinivasjois, Shreya Shah, Prakesh S. Shah, Knowledge Synthesis Group on Determinants of Preterm/LBW Births* |
895 |
|
-**Title:** *"Biracial Couples and Adverse Birth Outcomes: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis"* |
896 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1111/j.1600-0412.2012.01501.x](https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0412.2012.01501.x) |
897 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Neonatal Health, Maternal-Fetal Medicine, Racial Disparities* |
|
624 |
+{{expand title="Study: Is Homo sapiens polytypic? Human taxonomic diversity and its implications" expanded="false"}} |
|
625 |
+**Source:** *Medical Hypotheses (Elsevier)* |
|
626 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2010* |
|
627 |
+**Author(s):** *Michael A. Woodley* |
|
628 |
+**Title:** *"Is Homo sapiens polytypic? Human taxonomic diversity and its implications"* |
|
629 |
+**DOI:** [10.1016/j.mehy.2009.07.046](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2009.07.046) |
|
630 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Human Taxonomy, Evolutionary Biology, Anthropology* |
898 |
898 |
|
899 |
899 |
--- |
900 |
900 |
|
901 |
901 |
## **Key Statistics** |
902 |
902 |
1. **General Observations:** |
903 |
|
- - Meta-analysis of **26,335,596 singleton births** from eight studies. |
904 |
|
- - **Higher risk of adverse birth outcomes in biracial couples** than White couples, but lower than Black couples. |
|
636 |
+ - The study argues that **Homo sapiens is polytypic**, meaning it consists of multiple subspecies rather than a single monotypic species. |
|
637 |
+ - Examines **genetic diversity, morphological variation, and evolutionary lineage** in humans. |
905 |
905 |
|
906 |
906 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
907 |
|
- - **Maternal race had a stronger influence than paternal race** on birth outcomes. |
908 |
|
- - **Black mother–White father (BMWF) couples** had a higher risk than **White mother–Black father (WMBF) couples**. |
|
640 |
+ - Discusses **four primary definitions of race/subspecies**: Essentialist, Taxonomic, Population-based, and Lineage-based. |
|
641 |
+ - Suggests that **human heterozygosity levels are comparable to species that are classified as polytypic**. |
909 |
909 |
|
910 |
910 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
911 |
|
- - **Adjusted Odds Ratios (aORs) for key outcomes:** |
912 |
|
- - **Low birthweight (LBW):** WMBF (1.21), BMWF (1.75), Black mother–Black father (BMBF) (2.08). |
913 |
|
- - **Preterm births (PTB):** WMBF (1.17), BMWF (1.37), BMBF (1.78). |
914 |
|
- - **Stillbirths:** WMBF (1.43), BMWF (1.51), BMBF (1.85). |
|
644 |
+ - The study evaluates **FST values (genetic differentiation measure)** and argues that human genetic differentiation is comparable to that of recognized subspecies in other species. |
|
645 |
+ - Considers **phylogenetic species concepts** in defining human variation. |
915 |
915 |
|
916 |
916 |
--- |
917 |
917 |
|
918 |
918 |
## **Findings** |
919 |
919 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
920 |
|
- - **Biracial couples face a gradient of risk**: higher than White couples but lower than Black couples. |
921 |
|
- - **Maternal race plays a more significant role** in pregnancy outcomes. |
|
651 |
+ - Proposes that **modern human populations meet biological criteria for subspecies classification**. |
|
652 |
+ - Highlights **medical and evolutionary implications** of human taxonomic diversity. |
922 |
922 |
|
923 |
923 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
924 |
|
- - **Black mothers (regardless of paternal race) had the highest risk of LBW and PTB**. |
925 |
|
- - **White mothers with Black fathers had a lower risk** than Black mothers with White fathers. |
|
655 |
+ - Discusses **how race concepts evolved over time** in biological sciences. |
|
656 |
+ - Compares **human diversity with that of other primates** such as chimpanzees and gorillas. |
926 |
926 |
|
927 |
927 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
928 |
|
- - The **weathering hypothesis** suggests that **long-term stress exposure** contributes to higher adverse birth risks in Black mothers. |
929 |
|
- - **Genetic and environmental factors** may interact to influence birth outcomes. |
|
659 |
+ - Evaluates how **genetic markers correlate with population structure**. |
|
660 |
+ - Addresses the **controversy over race classification in modern anthropology**. |
930 |
930 |
|
931 |
931 |
--- |
932 |
932 |
|
933 |
933 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
934 |
934 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
935 |
|
- - **Largest meta-analysis** on racial disparities in birth outcomes. |
936 |
|
- - Uses **adjusted statistical models** to account for confounding variables. |
|
666 |
+ - Uses **comparative species analysis** to assess human classification. |
|
667 |
+ - Provides a **biological perspective** on the race concept, moving beyond social constructivism arguments. |
937 |
937 |
|
938 |
938 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
939 |
|
- - Data limited to **Black-White biracial couples**, excluding other racial groups. |
940 |
|
- - **Socioeconomic and healthcare access factors** not fully explored. |
|
670 |
+ - Controversial topic with **strong opposing views in anthropology and genetics**. |
|
671 |
+ - **Relies on broad genetic trends**, but does not analyze individual-level genetic variation in depth. |
941 |
941 |
|
942 |
942 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
943 |
|
- - Future studies should examine **Asian, Hispanic, and Indigenous biracial couples**. |
944 |
|
- - Investigate **long-term health effects on infants from biracial pregnancies**. |
|
674 |
+ - Further research should **incorporate whole-genome studies** to refine subspecies classifications. |
|
675 |
+ - Investigate **how admixture affects taxonomic classification over time**. |
945 |
945 |
|
946 |
946 |
--- |
947 |
947 |
|
948 |
948 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
949 |
|
-- Provides **critical insights into racial disparities** in maternal and infant health. |
950 |
|
-- Supports **research on genetic and environmental influences on neonatal health**. |
951 |
|
-- Highlights **how maternal race plays a more significant role than paternal race** in birth outcomes. |
|
680 |
+- Contributes to discussions on **evolutionary taxonomy and species classification**. |
|
681 |
+- Provides evidence on **genetic differentiation among human populations**. |
|
682 |
+- Highlights **historical and contemporary scientific debates on race and human variation**. |
952 |
952 |
|
953 |
953 |
--- |
954 |
954 |
|
955 |
955 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
956 |
|
-1. Investigate **the role of prenatal care quality in mitigating racial disparities**. |
957 |
|
-2. Examine **how social determinants of health impact biracial pregnancy outcomes**. |
958 |
|
-3. Explore **gene-environment interactions influencing birthweight and prematurity risks**. |
|
687 |
+1. Examine **FST values in modern and ancient human populations**. |
|
688 |
+2. Investigate how **adaptive evolution influences population differentiation**. |
|
689 |
+3. Explore **the impact of genetic diversity on medical treatments and disease susceptibility**. |
959 |
959 |
|
960 |
960 |
--- |
961 |
961 |
|
962 |
962 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
963 |
|
-This meta-analysis examines **the impact of biracial parentage on birth outcomes**, showing that **biracial couples face a higher risk of adverse pregnancy outcomes than White couples but lower than Black couples**. The findings emphasize **maternal race as a key factor in birth risks**, with **Black mothers having the highest rates of preterm birth and low birthweight, regardless of paternal race**. |
|
694 |
+This study evaluates **whether Homo sapiens should be classified as a polytypic species**, analyzing **genetic diversity, evolutionary lineage, and morphological variation**. Using comparative analysis with other primates and mammals, the research suggests that **human populations meet biological criteria for subspecies classification**, with implications for **evolutionary biology, anthropology, and medicine**. |
964 |
964 |
|
|
696 |
+This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
|
697 |
+ |
965 |
965 |
--- |
966 |
966 |
|
967 |
967 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
968 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1111_j.1600-0412.2012.01501.xAbstract.pdf]] |
|
701 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.mehy.2009.07.046.pdf]] |
969 |
969 |
|
970 |
970 |
{{/expand}} |
971 |
971 |
|
|
705 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
972 |
972 |
|
973 |
|
-== Study: One is the Loneliest Number: Involuntary Celibacy (Incel), Mental Health, and Loneliness == |
974 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: One is the Loneliest Number: Involuntary Celibacy (Incel), Mental Health, and Loneliness" expanded="false"}} |
975 |
|
-**Source:** *Current Psychology* |
976 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2024* |
977 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Brandon Sparks, Alexandra M. Zidenberg, Mark E. Olver* |
978 |
|
-**Title:** *"One is the Loneliest Number: Involuntary Celibacy (Incel), Mental Health, and Loneliness"* |
979 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1007/s12144-023-04275-z](https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04275-z) |
980 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Psychology, Mental Health, Social Isolation* |
|
707 |
+{{expand title="Study: The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age" expanded="false"}} |
|
708 |
+**Source:** *Twin Research and Human Genetics (Cambridge University Press)* |
|
709 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2013* |
|
710 |
+**Author(s):** *Thomas J. Bouchard Jr.* |
|
711 |
+**Title:** *"The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age"* |
|
712 |
+**DOI:** [10.1017/thg.2013.54](https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2013.54) |
|
713 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Intelligence, Heritability, Developmental Psychology* |
981 |
981 |
|
982 |
982 |
--- |
983 |
983 |
|
984 |
984 |
## **Key Statistics** |
985 |
985 |
1. **General Observations:** |
986 |
|
- - Study analyzed **67 self-identified incels** and **103 non-incel men**. |
987 |
|
- - Incels reported **higher loneliness and lower social support** compared to non-incels. |
|
719 |
+ - The study documents how the **heritability of IQ increases with age**, reaching an asymptote at **0.80 by adulthood**. |
|
720 |
+ - Analysis is based on **longitudinal twin and adoption studies**. |
988 |
988 |
|
989 |
989 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
990 |
|
- - Incels exhibited **higher levels of depression, anxiety, and self-critical rumination**. |
991 |
|
- - **Social isolation was a key factor** differentiating incels from non-incels. |
|
723 |
+ - Shared environmental influence on IQ **declines with age**, reaching **0.10 in adulthood**. |
|
724 |
+ - Monozygotic twins show **increasing genetic similarity in IQ over time**, while dizygotic twins become **less concordant**. |
992 |
992 |
|
993 |
993 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
994 |
|
- - 95% of incels in the study reported **having depression**, with 38% receiving a formal diagnosis. |
995 |
|
- - **Higher externalization of blame** was linked to stronger incel identification. |
|
727 |
+ - Data from the **Louisville Longitudinal Twin Study and cross-national twin samples** support findings. |
|
728 |
+ - IQ stability over time is **influenced more by genetics than by shared environmental factors**. |
996 |
996 |
|
997 |
997 |
--- |
998 |
998 |
|
999 |
999 |
## **Findings** |
1000 |
1000 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
1001 |
|
- - Incels experience **heightened rejection sensitivity and loneliness**. |
1002 |
|
- - Lack of social support correlates with **worse mental health outcomes**. |
|
734 |
+ - Intelligence heritability **strengthens throughout development**, contrary to early environmental models. |
|
735 |
+ - Shared environmental effects **decrease by late adolescence**, emphasizing **genetic influence in adulthood**. |
1003 |
1003 |
|
1004 |
1004 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1005 |
|
- - **Avoidant attachment styles** were a strong predictor of incel identity. |
1006 |
|
- - **Mate value perceptions** significantly differed between incels and non-incels. |
|
738 |
+ - Studies from **Scotland, Netherlands, and the US** show **consistent patterns of increasing heritability with age**. |
|
739 |
+ - Findings hold across **varied socio-economic and educational backgrounds**. |
1007 |
1007 |
|
1008 |
1008 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1009 |
|
- - Incels **engaged in fewer positive coping mechanisms** such as emotional support or positive reframing. |
1010 |
|
- - Instead, they relied on **solitary coping strategies**, worsening their isolation. |
|
742 |
+ - Longitudinal adoption studies show **declining impact of adoptive parental influence on IQ** as children age. |
|
743 |
+ - Cross-sectional twin data confirm **higher IQ correlations for monozygotic twins in adulthood**. |
1011 |
1011 |
|
1012 |
1012 |
--- |
1013 |
1013 |
|
1014 |
1014 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
1015 |
1015 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1016 |
|
- - **First quantitative study** on incels’ social isolation and mental health. |
1017 |
|
- - **Robust sample size** and validated psychological measures. |
|
749 |
+ - **Robust dataset covering multiple twin and adoption studies over decades**. |
|
750 |
+ - **Clear, replicable trend** demonstrating the increasing role of genetics in intelligence. |
1018 |
1018 |
|
1019 |
1019 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1020 |
|
- - Sample drawn from **Reddit communities**, which may not represent all incels. |
1021 |
|
- - **No causal conclusions**—correlations between isolation and inceldom need further research. |
|
753 |
+ - Findings apply primarily to **Western industrialized nations**, limiting generalizability. |
|
754 |
+ - **Lack of neurobiological mechanisms** explaining how genes express their influence over time. |
1022 |
1022 |
|
1023 |
1023 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1024 |
|
- - Future studies should **compare incel forum users vs. non-users**. |
1025 |
|
- - Investigate **potential intervention strategies** for social integration. |
|
757 |
+ - Future research should investigate **gene-environment interactions in cognitive aging**. |
|
758 |
+ - Examine **heritability trends in non-Western populations** to determine cross-cultural consistency. |
1026 |
1026 |
|
1027 |
1027 |
--- |
1028 |
1028 |
|
1029 |
1029 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
1030 |
|
-- Highlights **mental health vulnerabilities** within the incel community. |
1031 |
|
-- Supports research on **loneliness, attachment styles, and social dominance orientation**. |
1032 |
|
-- Examines how **peer rejection influences self-perceived mate value**. |
|
763 |
+- Provides **strong evidence for the genetic basis of intelligence**. |
|
764 |
+- Highlights the **diminishing role of shared environment in cognitive development**. |
|
765 |
+- Supports research on **cognitive aging and heritability across the lifespan**. |
1033 |
1033 |
|
1034 |
1034 |
--- |
1035 |
1035 |
|
1036 |
1036 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
1037 |
|
-1. Explore how **online community participation** affects incel mental health. |
1038 |
|
-2. Investigate **cognitive biases** influencing self-perceived rejection among incels. |
1039 |
|
-3. Assess **therapeutic interventions** to address incel social isolation. |
|
770 |
+1. Investigate **neurogenetic pathways underlying IQ development**. |
|
771 |
+2. Examine **how education and socioeconomic factors interact with genetic IQ influences**. |
|
772 |
+3. Study **heritability trends in aging populations and cognitive decline**. |
1040 |
1040 |
|
1041 |
1041 |
--- |
1042 |
1042 |
|
1043 |
1043 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
1044 |
|
-This study examines the **psychological characteristics of self-identified incels**, comparing them with non-incel men in terms of **mental health, loneliness, and coping strategies**. The research found **higher depression, anxiety, and avoidant attachment styles among incels**, as well as **greater reliance on solitary coping mechanisms**. It suggests that **lack of social support plays a critical role in exacerbating incel identity and related mental health concerns**. |
|
777 |
+This study documents **The Wilson Effect**, demonstrating how the **heritability of IQ increases throughout development**, reaching a plateau of **0.80 by adulthood**. The findings indicate that **shared environmental effects diminish with age**, while **genetic influences on intelligence strengthen**. Using **longitudinal twin and adoption data**, the research provides **strong empirical support for the increasing role of genetics in cognitive ability over time**. |
1045 |
1045 |
|
1046 |
1046 |
This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
1047 |
1047 |
|
... |
... |
@@ -1048,85 +1048,83 @@ |
1048 |
1048 |
--- |
1049 |
1049 |
|
1050 |
1050 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
1051 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1007_s12144-023-04275-z.pdf]] |
|
784 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1017_thg.2013.54.pdf]] |
1052 |
1052 |
|
1053 |
1053 |
{{/expand}} |
1054 |
1054 |
|
|
788 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
1055 |
1055 |
|
1056 |
|
-= Crime and Substance Abuse = |
|
790 |
+{{expand title="Study: Segregation, Innocence, and Protection: The Institutional Conditions That Maintain Whiteness in College Sports" expanded="false"}} |
|
791 |
+**Source:** *Journal of Diversity in Higher Education* |
|
792 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2019* |
|
793 |
+**Author(s):** *Kirsten Hextrum* |
|
794 |
+**Title:** *"Segregation, Innocence, and Protection: The Institutional Conditions That Maintain Whiteness in College Sports"* |
|
795 |
+**DOI:** [10.1037/dhe0000140](https://doi.org/10.1037/dhe0000140) |
|
796 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Race and Sports, Higher Education, Institutional Racism* |
1057 |
1057 |
|
1058 |
|
-== Study: Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys == |
1059 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys" expanded="false"}} |
1060 |
|
-**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
1061 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2003* |
1062 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Timothy P. Johnson, Phillip J. Bowman* |
1063 |
|
-**Title:** *"Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys"* |
1064 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120023394](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120023394) |
1065 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Survey Methodology, Racial Disparities, Substance Use Research* |
1066 |
|
- |
1067 |
1067 |
--- |
1068 |
1068 |
|
1069 |
1069 |
## **Key Statistics** |
1070 |
1070 |
1. **General Observations:** |
1071 |
|
- - Study examined **how racial and cultural factors influence self-reported substance use data**. |
1072 |
|
- - Analyzed **36 empirical studies from 1977–2003** on survey reliability across racial/ethnic groups. |
|
802 |
+ - Analyzed **47 college athlete narratives** to explore racial disparities in non-revenue sports. |
|
803 |
+ - Found three interrelated themes: **racial segregation, racial innocence, and racial protection**. |
1073 |
1073 |
|
1074 |
1074 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1075 |
|
- - Black and Latino respondents **were more likely to underreport drug use** compared to White respondents. |
1076 |
|
- - **Cultural stigma and distrust in research institutions** affected self-report accuracy. |
|
806 |
+ - **Predominantly white sports programs** reinforce racial hierarchies in college athletics. |
|
807 |
+ - **Recruitment policies favor white athletes** from affluent, suburban backgrounds. |
1077 |
1077 |
|
1078 |
1078 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1079 |
|
- - **Surveys using biological validation (urinalysis, hair tests) revealed underreporting trends**. |
1080 |
|
- - **Higher recantation rates** (denying past drug use) were observed among minority respondents. |
|
810 |
+ - White athletes are **socialized to remain unaware of racial privilege** in their athletic careers. |
|
811 |
+ - Media and institutional narratives protect white athletes from discussions on race and systemic inequities. |
1081 |
1081 |
|
1082 |
1082 |
--- |
1083 |
1083 |
|
1084 |
1084 |
## **Findings** |
1085 |
1085 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
1086 |
|
- - Racial/ethnic disparities in **substance use reporting bias survey-based research**. |
1087 |
|
- - **Social desirability and cultural norms impact data reliability**. |
|
817 |
+ - Colleges **actively recruit white athletes** from majority-white communities. |
|
818 |
+ - Institutional policies **uphold whiteness** by failing to challenge racial biases in recruitment and team culture. |
1088 |
1088 |
|
1089 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1090 |
|
- - White respondents were **more likely to overreport** substance use. |
1091 |
|
- - Black and Latino respondents **had higher recantation rates**, particularly in face-to-face interviews. |
|
820 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
821 |
+ - **White athletes show limited awareness** of their racial advantage in sports. |
|
822 |
+ - **Black athletes are overrepresented** in revenue-generating sports but underrepresented in non-revenue teams. |
1092 |
1092 |
|
1093 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1094 |
|
- - Mode of survey administration **significantly influenced reporting accuracy**. |
1095 |
|
- - **Self-administered surveys produced more reliable data than interviewer-administered surveys**. |
|
824 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
825 |
+ - Examines **how sports serve as a mechanism for maintaining racial privilege** in higher education. |
|
826 |
+ - Discusses the **role of athletics in reinforcing systemic segregation and exclusion**. |
1096 |
1096 |
|
1097 |
1097 |
--- |
1098 |
1098 |
|
1099 |
1099 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
1100 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1101 |
|
- - **Comprehensive review of 36 studies** on measurement error in substance use reporting. |
1102 |
|
- - Identifies **systemic biases affecting racial/ethnic survey reliability**. |
|
831 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
832 |
+ - **Comprehensive qualitative analysis** of race in college sports. |
|
833 |
+ - Examines **institutional conditions** that sustain racial disparities in athletics. |
1103 |
1103 |
|
1104 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1105 |
|
- - Relies on **secondary data analysis**, limiting direct experimental control. |
1106 |
|
- - Does not explore **how measurement error impacts policy decisions**. |
|
835 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
836 |
+ - Focuses primarily on **Division I non-revenue sports**, limiting generalizability to other divisions. |
|
837 |
+ - Lacks extensive **quantitative data on racial demographics** in college athletics. |
1107 |
1107 |
|
1108 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1109 |
|
- - Future research should **incorporate mixed-method approaches** (qualitative & quantitative). |
1110 |
|
- - Investigate **how survey design can reduce racial reporting disparities**. |
|
839 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
840 |
+ - Future research should **compare recruitment policies across different sports and divisions**. |
|
841 |
+ - Investigate **how athletic scholarships contribute to racial inequities in higher education**. |
1111 |
1111 |
|
1112 |
1112 |
--- |
1113 |
1113 |
|
1114 |
1114 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
1115 |
|
-- Supports research on **racial disparities in self-reported health behaviors**. |
1116 |
|
-- Highlights **survey methodology issues that impact substance use epidemiology**. |
1117 |
|
-- Provides insights for **improving data accuracy in public health research**. |
|
846 |
+- Provides evidence of **systemic racial biases** in college sports recruitment. |
|
847 |
+- Highlights **how institutional policies protect whiteness** in non-revenue athletics. |
|
848 |
+- Supports research on **diversity, equity, and inclusion (DEI) efforts in sports and education**. |
1118 |
1118 |
|
1119 |
1119 |
--- |
1120 |
1120 |
|
1121 |
1121 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
1122 |
|
-1. Investigate **how survey design impacts racial disparities in self-reported health data**. |
1123 |
|
-2. Study **alternative data collection methods (biometric validation, passive data tracking)**. |
1124 |
|
-3. Explore **the role of social stigma in self-reported health behaviors**. |
|
853 |
+1. Investigate how **racial stereotypes influence college athlete recruitment**. |
|
854 |
+2. Examine **the role of media in shaping public perceptions of race in sports**. |
|
855 |
+3. Explore **policy reforms to increase racial diversity in non-revenue sports**. |
1125 |
1125 |
|
1126 |
1126 |
--- |
1127 |
1127 |
|
1128 |
1128 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
1129 |
|
-This study examines **cross-cultural biases in self-reported substance use surveys**, showing that **racial/ethnic minorities are more likely to underreport drug use** due to **social stigma, research distrust, and survey administration methods**. The findings highlight **critical issues in public health data collection and the need for improved survey design**. |
|
860 |
+This study explores how **racial segregation, innocence, and protection** sustain whiteness in college sports. By analyzing **47 athlete narratives**, the research reveals **how predominantly white sports programs recruit and retain white athletes** while shielding them from discussions on race. The findings highlight **institutional biases that maintain racial privilege in athletics**, offering critical insight into the **structural inequalities in higher education sports programs**. |
1130 |
1130 |
|
1131 |
1131 |
This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
1132 |
1132 |
|
... |
... |
@@ -1133,82 +1133,83 @@ |
1133 |
1133 |
--- |
1134 |
1134 |
|
1135 |
1135 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
1136 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120023394.pdf]] |
|
867 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1037_dhe0000140.pdf]] |
1137 |
1137 |
|
1138 |
1138 |
{{/expand}} |
1139 |
1139 |
|
1140 |
|
-== Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program == |
1141 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program" expanded="false"}} |
1142 |
|
-**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
1143 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2002* |
1144 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Clifford A. Butzin, Christine A. Saum, Frank R. Scarpitti* |
1145 |
|
-**Title:** *"Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"* |
1146 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120014424](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120014424) |
1147 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Substance Use, Criminal Justice, Drug Courts* |
|
871 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
1148 |
1148 |
|
|
873 |
+{{expand title="Study: Reconstructing Indian Population History" expanded="false"}} |
|
874 |
+**Source:** *Nature* |
|
875 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2009* |
|
876 |
+**Author(s):** *David Reich, Kumarasamy Thangaraj, Nick Patterson, Alkes L. Price, Lalji Singh* |
|
877 |
+**Title:** *"Reconstructing Indian Population History"* |
|
878 |
+**DOI:** [10.1038/nature08365](https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08365) |
|
879 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Genetics, Population History, South Asian Ancestry* |
|
880 |
+ |
1149 |
1149 |
--- |
1150 |
1150 |
|
1151 |
1151 |
## **Key Statistics** |
1152 |
1152 |
1. **General Observations:** |
1153 |
|
- - Study examined **drug treatment court success rates** among first-time offenders. |
1154 |
|
- - Strongest predictors of **successful completion were employment status and race**. |
|
885 |
+ - Study analyzed **132 individuals from 25 diverse Indian groups**. |
|
886 |
+ - Identified two major ancestral populations: **Ancestral North Indians (ANI)** and **Ancestral South Indians (ASI)**. |
1155 |
1155 |
|
1156 |
1156 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1157 |
|
- - Individuals with **stable jobs were more likely to complete the program**. |
1158 |
|
- - **Black participants had lower success rates**, suggesting potential systemic disparities. |
|
889 |
+ - ANI ancestry is closely related to **Middle Easterners, Central Asians, and Europeans**. |
|
890 |
+ - ASI ancestry is **genetically distinct from ANI and East Asians**. |
1159 |
1159 |
|
1160 |
1160 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1161 |
|
- - **Education level was positively correlated** with program completion. |
1162 |
|
- - Frequency of **drug use before enrollment affected treatment outcomes**. |
|
893 |
+ - ANI ancestry ranges from **39% to 71%** across Indian groups. |
|
894 |
+ - **Caste and linguistic differences** strongly correlate with genetic variation. |
1163 |
1163 |
|
1164 |
1164 |
--- |
1165 |
1165 |
|
1166 |
1166 |
## **Findings** |
1167 |
1167 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
1168 |
|
- - **Social stability factors** (employment, education) were key to treatment success. |
1169 |
|
- - **Race and pre-existing substance use patterns** influenced completion rates. |
|
900 |
+ - The genetic landscape of India has been shaped by **thousands of years of endogamy**. |
|
901 |
+ - Groups with **only ASI ancestry no longer exist** in mainland India. |
1170 |
1170 |
|
1171 |
1171 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1172 |
|
- - White offenders had **higher completion rates** than Black offenders. |
1173 |
|
- - Drug court success was **higher for those with lower initial drug use frequency**. |
|
904 |
+ - **Higher ANI ancestry in upper-caste and Indo-European-speaking groups**. |
|
905 |
+ - **Andaman Islanders** are unique in having **ASI ancestry without ANI influence**. |
1174 |
1174 |
|
1175 |
1175 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1176 |
|
- - **Individuals with strong social ties were more likely to finish the program**. |
1177 |
|
- - Success rates were **significantly higher for participants with case management support**. |
|
908 |
+ - **Founder effects** have maintained allele frequency differences among Indian groups. |
|
909 |
+ - Predicts **higher incidence of recessive diseases** due to historical genetic isolation. |
1178 |
1178 |
|
1179 |
1179 |
--- |
1180 |
1180 |
|
1181 |
1181 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
1182 |
1182 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1183 |
|
- - **First empirical study on drug court program success factors**. |
1184 |
|
- - Uses **longitudinal data** for post-treatment analysis. |
|
915 |
+ - **First large-scale genetic analysis** of Indian population history. |
|
916 |
+ - Introduces **new methods for ancestry estimation without direct ancestral reference groups**. |
1185 |
1185 |
|
1186 |
1186 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1187 |
|
- - Lacks **qualitative data on personal motivation and treatment engagement**. |
1188 |
|
- - Focuses on **short-term program success** without tracking **long-term relapse rates**. |
|
919 |
+ - Limited **sample size relative to India's population diversity**. |
|
920 |
+ - Does not include **recent admixture events** post-colonial era. |
1189 |
1189 |
|
1190 |
1190 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1191 |
|
- - Future research should examine **racial disparities in drug court outcomes**. |
1192 |
|
- - Study **how community resources impact long-term recovery**. |
|
923 |
+ - Future research should **expand sampling across more Indian tribal groups**. |
|
924 |
+ - Use **whole-genome sequencing** for finer resolution of ancestry. |
1193 |
1193 |
|
1194 |
1194 |
--- |
1195 |
1195 |
|
1196 |
1196 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
1197 |
|
-- Provides insight into **what factors contribute to drug court program success**. |
1198 |
|
-- Highlights **racial disparities in criminal justice-based rehabilitation programs**. |
1199 |
|
-- Supports **policy discussions on improving access to drug treatment for marginalized groups**. |
|
929 |
+- Provides a **genetic basis for caste and linguistic diversity** in India. |
|
930 |
+- Highlights **founder effects and genetic drift** shaping South Asian populations. |
|
931 |
+- Supports research on **medical genetics and disease risk prediction** in Indian populations. |
1200 |
1200 |
|
1201 |
1201 |
--- |
1202 |
1202 |
|
1203 |
1203 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
1204 |
|
-1. Investigate **the role of mental health in drug court success rates**. |
1205 |
|
-2. Assess **long-term relapse prevention strategies post-treatment**. |
1206 |
|
-3. Explore **alternative diversion programs beyond traditional drug courts**. |
|
936 |
+1. Examine **genetic markers linked to disease susceptibility** in Indian subpopulations. |
|
937 |
+2. Investigate the impact of **recent migration patterns on ANI-ASI ancestry distribution**. |
|
938 |
+3. Study **gene flow between Indian populations and other global groups**. |
1207 |
1207 |
|
1208 |
1208 |
--- |
1209 |
1209 |
|
1210 |
1210 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
1211 |
|
-This study examines **factors influencing the completion of drug treatment court programs**, identifying **employment, education, and race as key predictors**. The research underscores **systemic disparities in drug court outcomes**, emphasizing the need for **improved support systems for at-risk populations**. |
|
943 |
+This study reconstructs **the genetic history of India**, revealing two ancestral populations—**ANI (related to West Eurasians) and ASI (distinctly South Asian)**. By analyzing **25 diverse Indian groups**, the researchers demonstrate how **historical endogamy and founder effects** have maintained genetic differentiation. The findings have **implications for medical genetics, population history, and the study of South Asian ancestry**. |
1212 |
1212 |
|
1213 |
1213 |
This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
1214 |
1214 |
|
... |
... |
@@ -1215,165 +1215,84 @@ |
1215 |
1215 |
--- |
1216 |
1216 |
|
1217 |
1217 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
1218 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120014424.pdf]] |
|
950 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nature08365.pdf]] |
1219 |
1219 |
|
1220 |
1220 |
{{/expand}} |
1221 |
1221 |
|
1222 |
|
-== Study: Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys == |
1223 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys" expanded="false"}} |
1224 |
|
-**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
1225 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2003* |
1226 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Timothy P. Johnson, Phillip J. Bowman* |
1227 |
|
-**Title:** *"Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys"* |
1228 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120023394](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120023394) |
1229 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Survey Methodology, Racial Disparities, Substance Use Research* |
|
954 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
1230 |
1230 |
|
1231 |
|
---- |
1232 |
1232 |
|
1233 |
|
-## **Key Statistics** |
1234 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1235 |
|
- - Study examined **how racial and cultural factors influence self-reported substance use data**. |
1236 |
|
- - Analyzed **36 empirical studies from 1977–2003** on survey reliability across racial/ethnic groups. |
|
957 |
+{{expand title="Study: The Simons Genome Diversity Project: 300 Genomes from 142 Diverse Populations" expanded="false"}} |
|
958 |
+**Source:** *Nature* |
|
959 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2016* |
|
960 |
+**Author(s):** *David Reich, Swapan Mallick, Heng Li, Mark Lipson, and others* |
|
961 |
+**Title:** *"The Simons Genome Diversity Project: 300 Genomes from 142 Diverse Populations"* |
|
962 |
+**DOI:** [10.1038/nature18964](https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18964) |
|
963 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Human Genetic Diversity, Population History, Evolutionary Genomics* |
1237 |
1237 |
|
1238 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1239 |
|
- - Black and Latino respondents **were more likely to underreport drug use** compared to White respondents. |
1240 |
|
- - **Cultural stigma and distrust in research institutions** affected self-report accuracy. |
1241 |
|
- |
1242 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1243 |
|
- - **Surveys using biological validation (urinalysis, hair tests) revealed underreporting trends**. |
1244 |
|
- - **Higher recantation rates** (denying past drug use) were observed among minority respondents. |
1245 |
|
- |
1246 |
1246 |
--- |
1247 |
1247 |
|
1248 |
|
-## **Findings** |
1249 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1250 |
|
- - Racial/ethnic disparities in **substance use reporting bias survey-based research**. |
1251 |
|
- - **Social desirability and cultural norms impact data reliability**. |
1252 |
|
- |
1253 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1254 |
|
- - White respondents were **more likely to overreport** substance use. |
1255 |
|
- - Black and Latino respondents **had higher recantation rates**, particularly in face-to-face interviews. |
1256 |
|
- |
1257 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1258 |
|
- - Mode of survey administration **significantly influenced reporting accuracy**. |
1259 |
|
- - **Self-administered surveys produced more reliable data than interviewer-administered surveys**. |
1260 |
|
- |
1261 |
|
---- |
1262 |
|
- |
1263 |
|
-## **Critique and Observations** |
1264 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1265 |
|
- - **Comprehensive review of 36 studies** on measurement error in substance use reporting. |
1266 |
|
- - Identifies **systemic biases affecting racial/ethnic survey reliability**. |
1267 |
|
- |
1268 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1269 |
|
- - Relies on **secondary data analysis**, limiting direct experimental control. |
1270 |
|
- - Does not explore **how measurement error impacts policy decisions**. |
1271 |
|
- |
1272 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1273 |
|
- - Future research should **incorporate mixed-method approaches** (qualitative & quantitative). |
1274 |
|
- - Investigate **how survey design can reduce racial reporting disparities**. |
1275 |
|
- |
1276 |
|
---- |
1277 |
|
- |
1278 |
|
-## **Relevance to Subproject** |
1279 |
|
-- Supports research on **racial disparities in self-reported health behaviors**. |
1280 |
|
-- Highlights **survey methodology issues that impact substance use epidemiology**. |
1281 |
|
-- Provides insights for **improving data accuracy in public health research**. |
1282 |
|
- |
1283 |
|
---- |
1284 |
|
- |
1285 |
|
-## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
1286 |
|
-1. Investigate **how survey design impacts racial disparities in self-reported health data**. |
1287 |
|
-2. Study **alternative data collection methods (biometric validation, passive data tracking)**. |
1288 |
|
-3. Explore **the role of social stigma in self-reported health behaviors**. |
1289 |
|
- |
1290 |
|
---- |
1291 |
|
- |
1292 |
|
-## **Summary of Research Study** |
1293 |
|
-This study examines **cross-cultural biases in self-reported substance use surveys**, showing that **racial/ethnic minorities are more likely to underreport drug use** due to **social stigma, research distrust, and survey administration methods**. The findings highlight **critical issues in public health data collection and the need for improved survey design**. |
1294 |
|
- |
1295 |
|
-This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
1296 |
|
- |
1297 |
|
---- |
1298 |
|
- |
1299 |
|
-## **📄 Download Full Study** |
1300 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120023394.pdf]] |
1301 |
|
- |
1302 |
|
-{{/expand}} |
1303 |
|
- |
1304 |
|
- |
1305 |
|
-== Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program == |
1306 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program" expanded="false"}} |
1307 |
|
-**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
1308 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2002* |
1309 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Clifford A. Butzin, Christine A. Saum, Frank R. Scarpitti* |
1310 |
|
-**Title:** *"Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"* |
1311 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120014424](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120014424) |
1312 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Substance Use, Criminal Justice, Drug Courts* |
1313 |
|
- |
1314 |
|
---- |
1315 |
|
- |
1316 |
1316 |
## **Key Statistics** |
1317 |
1317 |
1. **General Observations:** |
1318 |
|
- - Study examined **drug treatment court success rates** among first-time offenders. |
1319 |
|
- - Strongest predictors of **successful completion were employment status and race**. |
|
969 |
+ - Analyzed **high-coverage genome sequences of 300 individuals from 142 populations**. |
|
970 |
+ - Included **many underrepresented and indigenous groups** from Africa, Asia, Europe, and the Americas. |
1320 |
1320 |
|
1321 |
1321 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1322 |
|
- - Individuals with **stable jobs were more likely to complete the program**. |
1323 |
|
- - **Black participants had lower success rates**, suggesting potential systemic disparities. |
|
973 |
+ - Found **higher genetic diversity within African populations** compared to non-African groups. |
|
974 |
+ - Showed **Neanderthal and Denisovan ancestry in non-African populations**, particularly in Oceania. |
1324 |
1324 |
|
1325 |
1325 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1326 |
|
- - **Education level was positively correlated** with program completion. |
1327 |
|
- - Frequency of **drug use before enrollment affected treatment outcomes**. |
|
977 |
+ - Identified **5.8 million base pairs absent from the human reference genome**. |
|
978 |
+ - Estimated that **mutations have accumulated 5% faster in non-Africans than in Africans**. |
1328 |
1328 |
|
1329 |
1329 |
--- |
1330 |
1330 |
|
1331 |
1331 |
## **Findings** |
1332 |
1332 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
1333 |
|
- - **Social stability factors** (employment, education) were key to treatment success. |
1334 |
|
- - **Race and pre-existing substance use patterns** influenced completion rates. |
|
984 |
+ - **African populations harbor the greatest genetic diversity**, confirming an out-of-Africa dispersal model. |
|
985 |
+ - Indigenous Australians and New Guineans **share a common ancestral population with other non-Africans**. |
1335 |
1335 |
|
1336 |
1336 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1337 |
|
- - White offenders had **higher completion rates** than Black offenders. |
1338 |
|
- - Drug court success was **higher for those with lower initial drug use frequency**. |
|
988 |
+ - **Lower heterozygosity in non-Africans** due to founder effects from migration bottlenecks. |
|
989 |
+ - **Denisovan ancestry in South Asians is higher than previously thought**. |
1339 |
1339 |
|
1340 |
1340 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1341 |
|
- - **Individuals with strong social ties were more likely to finish the program**. |
1342 |
|
- - Success rates were **significantly higher for participants with case management support**. |
|
992 |
+ - **Neanderthal ancestry is higher in East Asians than in Europeans**. |
|
993 |
+ - African hunter-gatherer groups show **deep population splits over 100,000 years ago**. |
1343 |
1343 |
|
1344 |
1344 |
--- |
1345 |
1345 |
|
1346 |
1346 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
1347 |
1347 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1348 |
|
- - **First empirical study on drug court program success factors**. |
1349 |
|
- - Uses **longitudinal data** for post-treatment analysis. |
|
999 |
+ - **Largest global genetic dataset** outside of the 1000 Genomes Project. |
|
1000 |
+ - High sequencing depth allows **more accurate identification of genetic variants**. |
1350 |
1350 |
|
1351 |
1351 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1352 |
|
- - Lacks **qualitative data on personal motivation and treatment engagement**. |
1353 |
|
- - Focuses on **short-term program success** without tracking **long-term relapse rates**. |
|
1003 |
+ - **Limited sample sizes for some populations**, restricting generalizability. |
|
1004 |
+ - Lacks ancient DNA comparisons, making it difficult to reconstruct deep ancestry fully. |
1354 |
1354 |
|
1355 |
1355 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1356 |
|
- - Future research should examine **racial disparities in drug court outcomes**. |
1357 |
|
- - Study **how community resources impact long-term recovery**. |
|
1007 |
+ - Future studies should include **ancient genomes** to improve demographic modeling. |
|
1008 |
+ - Expand research into **how genetic variation affects health outcomes** across populations. |
1358 |
1358 |
|
1359 |
1359 |
--- |
1360 |
1360 |
|
1361 |
1361 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
1362 |
|
-- Provides insight into **what factors contribute to drug court program success**. |
1363 |
|
-- Highlights **racial disparities in criminal justice-based rehabilitation programs**. |
1364 |
|
-- Supports **policy discussions on improving access to drug treatment for marginalized groups**. |
|
1013 |
+- Provides **comprehensive data on human genetic diversity**, useful for **evolutionary studies**. |
|
1014 |
+- Supports research on **Neanderthal and Denisovan introgression** in modern human populations. |
|
1015 |
+- Enhances understanding of **genetic adaptation and disease susceptibility across groups**. |
1365 |
1365 |
|
1366 |
1366 |
--- |
1367 |
1367 |
|
1368 |
1368 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
1369 |
|
-1. Investigate **the role of mental health in drug court success rates**. |
1370 |
|
-2. Assess **long-term relapse prevention strategies post-treatment**. |
1371 |
|
-3. Explore **alternative diversion programs beyond traditional drug courts**. |
|
1020 |
+1. Investigate **functional consequences of genetic variation in underrepresented populations**. |
|
1021 |
+2. Study **how selection pressures shaped genetic diversity across different environments**. |
|
1022 |
+3. Explore **medical applications of population-specific genetic markers**. |
1372 |
1372 |
|
1373 |
1373 |
--- |
1374 |
1374 |
|
1375 |
1375 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
1376 |
|
-This study examines **factors influencing the completion of drug treatment court programs**, identifying **employment, education, and race as key predictors**. The research underscores **systemic disparities in drug court outcomes**, emphasizing the need for **improved support systems for at-risk populations**. |
|
1027 |
+This study presents **high-coverage genome sequences from 300 individuals across 142 populations**, offering **new insights into global genetic diversity and human evolution**. The findings highlight **deep African population splits, widespread archaic ancestry in non-Africans, and unique variants absent from the human reference genome**. The research enhances our understanding of **migration patterns, adaptation, and evolutionary history**. |
1377 |
1377 |
|
1378 |
1378 |
This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
1379 |
1379 |
|
... |
... |
@@ -1380,148 +1380,83 @@ |
1380 |
1380 |
--- |
1381 |
1381 |
|
1382 |
1382 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
1383 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120014424.pdf]] |
|
1034 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nature18964.pdf]] |
1384 |
1384 |
|
1385 |
1385 |
{{/expand}} |
1386 |
1386 |
|
1387 |
|
-== Study: Associations Between Cannabis Use and Mental Health Symptoms in Young Adults == |
1388 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Associations Between Cannabis Use and Mental Health Symptoms in Young Adults" expanded="false"}} Source: Addictive Behaviors |
1389 |
|
-Date of Publication: 2016 |
1390 |
|
-Author(s): Andrea Hussong, Christy Capron, Gregory T. Smith, Jennifer L. Maggs |
1391 |
|
-Title: "Associations Between Cannabis Use and Mental Health Symptoms in Young Adults" |
1392 |
|
-DOI: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.02.030 |
1393 |
|
-Subject Matter: Substance Use, Mental Health, Adolescent Development |
|
1038 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
1394 |
1394 |
|
1395 |
|
-Key Statistics |
1396 |
|
-General Observations: |
|
1040 |
+{{expand title="Study: Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies" expanded="false"}} |
|
1041 |
+**Source:** *Nature Genetics* |
|
1042 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2015* |
|
1043 |
+**Author(s):** *Tinca J. C. Polderman, Beben Benyamin, Christiaan A. de Leeuw, Patrick F. Sullivan, Arjen van Bochoven, Peter M. Visscher, Danielle Posthuma* |
|
1044 |
+**Title:** *"Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies"* |
|
1045 |
+**DOI:** [10.1038/ng.328](https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.328) |
|
1046 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Genetics, Heritability, Twin Studies, Behavioral Science* |
1397 |
1397 |
|
1398 |
|
-Study examined cannabis use trends in young adults over time. |
1399 |
|
-Found significant correlations between cannabis use and increased depressive symptoms. |
1400 |
|
-Subgroup Analysis: |
1401 |
|
- |
1402 |
|
-Males exhibited higher rates of cannabis use, but females reported stronger mental health impacts. |
1403 |
|
-Individuals with pre-existing anxiety disorders were more likely to report problematic cannabis use. |
1404 |
|
-Other Significant Data Points: |
1405 |
|
- |
1406 |
|
-Frequent cannabis users showed a 23% higher likelihood of developing anxiety symptoms. |
1407 |
|
-Co-occurring substance use (e.g., alcohol) exacerbated negative psychological effects. |
1408 |
|
-Findings |
1409 |
|
-Primary Observations: |
1410 |
|
- |
1411 |
|
-Cannabis use was linked to higher depressive and anxiety symptoms, particularly in frequent users. |
1412 |
|
-Self-medication patterns emerged among those with pre-existing mental health conditions. |
1413 |
|
-Subgroup Trends: |
1414 |
|
- |
1415 |
|
-Early cannabis initiation (before age 16) was associated with greater mental health risks. |
1416 |
|
-College-aged users reported more impairments in daily functioning due to cannabis use. |
1417 |
|
-Specific Case Analysis: |
1418 |
|
- |
1419 |
|
-Participants with a history of childhood trauma were twice as likely to develop problematic cannabis use. |
1420 |
|
-Co-use of cannabis and alcohol significantly increased impulsivity scores in the study sample. |
1421 |
|
-Critique and Observations |
1422 |
|
-Strengths of the Study: |
1423 |
|
- |
1424 |
|
-Large, longitudinal dataset with a diverse sample of young adults. |
1425 |
|
-Controlled for confounding variables like socioeconomic status and prior substance use. |
1426 |
|
-Limitations of the Study: |
1427 |
|
- |
1428 |
|
-Self-reported cannabis use may introduce bias in reported frequency and effects. |
1429 |
|
-Did not assess specific THC potency levels, which could influence mental health outcomes. |
1430 |
|
-Suggestions for Improvement: |
1431 |
|
- |
1432 |
|
-Future research should investigate dose-dependent effects of cannabis on mental health. |
1433 |
|
-Assess long-term psychological outcomes of early cannabis exposure. |
1434 |
|
-Relevance to Subproject |
1435 |
|
-Supports mental health risk assessment models related to substance use. |
1436 |
|
-Highlights gender differences in substance-related psychological impacts. |
1437 |
|
-Provides insight into self-medication behaviors among young adults. |
1438 |
|
-Suggestions for Further Exploration |
1439 |
|
-Investigate the long-term impact of cannabis use on neurodevelopment. |
1440 |
|
-Examine the role of genetic predisposition in cannabis-related mental health risks. |
1441 |
|
-Assess regional differences in cannabis use trends post-legalization. |
1442 |
|
-Summary of Research Study |
1443 |
|
-This study examines the relationship between cannabis use and mental health symptoms in young adults, focusing on depressive and anxiety-related outcomes. Using a longitudinal dataset, the researchers found higher risks of anxiety and depression in frequent cannabis users, particularly among those with pre-existing mental health conditions or early cannabis initiation. |
1444 |
|
- |
1445 |
|
-This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
1446 |
|
- |
1447 |
|
-📄 Download Full Study |
1448 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.addbeh.2016.02.030.pdf]] |
1449 |
|
- |
1450 |
|
-{{/expand}} |
1451 |
|
- |
1452 |
|
- |
1453 |
|
-== Study: Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time? == |
1454 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time?" expanded="false"}} |
1455 |
|
-**Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)* |
1456 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2014* |
1457 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Michael A. Woodley, Jan te Nijenhuis, Raegan Murphy* |
1458 |
|
-**Title:** *"Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time?"* |
1459 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.012](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.012) |
1460 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Cognitive Decline, Intelligence, Dysgenics* |
1461 |
|
- |
1462 |
1462 |
--- |
1463 |
1463 |
|
1464 |
1464 |
## **Key Statistics** |
1465 |
1465 |
1. **General Observations:** |
1466 |
|
- - The study examines reaction time data from **13 age-matched studies** spanning **1884–2004**. |
1467 |
|
- - Results suggest an estimated **decline of 13.35 IQ points** over this period. |
|
1052 |
+ - Analyzed **17,804 traits from 2,748 twin studies** published between **1958 and 2012**. |
|
1053 |
+ - Included data from **14,558,903 twin pairs**, making it the largest meta-analysis on human heritability. |
1468 |
1468 |
|
1469 |
1469 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1470 |
|
- - The study found **slower reaction times in modern populations** compared to Victorian-era individuals. |
1471 |
|
- - Data from **Western countries (US, UK, Canada, Australia, Finland)** were analyzed. |
|
1056 |
+ - Found **49% average heritability** across all traits. |
|
1057 |
+ - **69% of traits follow a simple additive genetic model**, meaning most variance is due to genes, not environment. |
1472 |
1472 |
|
1473 |
1473 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1474 |
|
- - The estimated **dysgenic rate is 1.21 IQ points lost per decade**. |
1475 |
|
- - Meta-regression analysis confirmed a **steady secular trend in slowing reaction time**. |
|
1060 |
+ - **Neurological, metabolic, and psychiatric traits** showed the highest heritability estimates. |
|
1061 |
+ - Traits related to **social values and environmental interactions** had lower heritability estimates. |
1476 |
1476 |
|
1477 |
1477 |
--- |
1478 |
1478 |
|
1479 |
1479 |
## **Findings** |
1480 |
1480 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
1481 |
|
- - Supports the hypothesis of **intelligence decline due to genetic and environmental factors**. |
1482 |
|
- - Reaction time, a **biomarker for cognitive ability**, has slowed significantly over time. |
|
1067 |
+ - Across all traits, genetic factors play a significant role in individual differences. |
|
1068 |
+ - The study contradicts models that **overestimate environmental effects in behavioral and cognitive traits**. |
1483 |
1483 |
|
1484 |
1484 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1485 |
|
- - A stronger **correlation between slower reaction time and lower general intelligence (g)**. |
1486 |
|
- - Flynn effect (IQ gains) does not contradict this finding, as reaction time is a **biological, not environmental, measure**. |
|
1071 |
+ - **Eye and brain-related traits showed the highest heritability (~70-80%)**. |
|
1072 |
+ - **Shared environmental effects were negligible (<10%) for most traits**. |
1487 |
1487 |
|
1488 |
1488 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1489 |
|
- - Cross-national comparisons indicate a **global trend in slower reaction times**. |
1490 |
|
- - Factors like **modern neurotoxin exposure** and **reduced selective pressure for intelligence** may contribute. |
|
1075 |
+ - Twin correlations suggest **limited evidence for strong non-additive genetic influences**. |
|
1076 |
+ - The study highlights **missing heritability in complex traits**, which genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have yet to fully explain. |
1491 |
1491 |
|
1492 |
1492 |
--- |
1493 |
1493 |
|
1494 |
1494 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
1495 |
1495 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1496 |
|
- - **Comprehensive meta-analysis** covering over a century of reaction time data. |
1497 |
|
- - **Robust statistical corrections** for measurement variance between historical and modern studies. |
|
1082 |
+ - **Largest-ever heritability meta-analysis**, covering nearly all published twin studies. |
|
1083 |
+ - Provides a **comprehensive framework for understanding gene-environment contributions**. |
1498 |
1498 |
|
1499 |
1499 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1500 |
|
- - Some historical data sources **lack methodological consistency**. |
1501 |
|
- - **Reaction time measurements vary by study**, requiring adjustments for equipment differences. |
|
1086 |
+ - **Underrepresentation of African, South American, and Asian twin cohorts**, limiting global generalizability. |
|
1087 |
+ - Cannot **fully separate genetic influences from potential cultural/environmental confounders**. |
1502 |
1502 |
|
1503 |
1503 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1504 |
|
- - Future studies should **replicate results with more modern datasets**. |
1505 |
|
- - Investigate **alternative cognitive biomarkers** for intelligence over time. |
|
1090 |
+ - Future research should use **whole-genome sequencing** for finer-grained heritability estimates. |
|
1091 |
+ - **Incorporate non-Western populations** to assess global heritability trends. |
1506 |
1506 |
|
1507 |
1507 |
--- |
1508 |
1508 |
|
1509 |
1509 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
1510 |
|
-- Provides evidence for **long-term intelligence trends**, contributing to research on **cognitive evolution**. |
1511 |
|
-- Aligns with broader discussions on **dysgenics, neurophysiology, and cognitive load**. |
1512 |
|
-- Supports the argument that **modern societies may be experiencing intelligence decline**. |
|
1096 |
+- Establishes a **quantitative benchmark for heritability across human traits**. |
|
1097 |
+- Reinforces **genetic influence on cognitive, behavioral, and physical traits**. |
|
1098 |
+- Highlights the need for **genome-wide studies to identify missing heritability**. |
1513 |
1513 |
|
1514 |
1514 |
--- |
1515 |
1515 |
|
1516 |
1516 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
1517 |
|
-1. Investigate **genetic markers associated with reaction time** and intelligence decline. |
1518 |
|
-2. Examine **regional variations in reaction time trends**. |
1519 |
|
-3. Explore **cognitive resilience factors that counteract the decline**. |
|
1103 |
+1. Investigate how **heritability estimates compare across different socioeconomic backgrounds**. |
|
1104 |
+2. Examine **gene-environment interactions in cognitive and psychiatric traits**. |
|
1105 |
+3. Explore **non-additive genetic effects on human traits using newer statistical models**. |
1520 |
1520 |
|
1521 |
1521 |
--- |
1522 |
1522 |
|
1523 |
1523 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
1524 |
|
-This study examines **historical reaction time data** as a measure of **cognitive ability and intelligence decline**, analyzing data from **Western populations between 1884 and 2004**. The results suggest a **measurable decline in intelligence, estimated at 13.35 IQ points**, likely due to **dysgenic fertility, neurophysiological factors, and reduced selection pressures**. |
|
1110 |
+This study presents a **comprehensive meta-analysis of human trait heritability**, covering **over 50 years of twin research**. The findings confirm **genes play a predominant role in shaping human traits**, with an **average heritability of 49%** across all measured characteristics. The research offers **valuable insights into genetic and environmental influences**, guiding future gene-mapping efforts and behavioral genetics studies. |
1525 |
1525 |
|
1526 |
1526 |
This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
1527 |
1527 |
|
... |
... |
@@ -1528,88 +1528,83 @@ |
1528 |
1528 |
--- |
1529 |
1529 |
|
1530 |
1530 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
1531 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2014.05.012.pdf]] |
|
1117 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_ng.328.pdf]] |
1532 |
1532 |
|
1533 |
1533 |
{{/expand}} |
1534 |
1534 |
|
|
1121 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
1535 |
1535 |
|
|
1123 |
+{{expand title="Study: Genetic Analysis of African Populations: Human Evolution and Complex Disease" expanded="false"}} |
|
1124 |
+**Source:** *Nature Reviews Genetics* |
|
1125 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2002* |
|
1126 |
+**Author(s):** *Sarah A. Tishkoff, Scott M. Williams* |
|
1127 |
+**Title:** *"Genetic Analysis of African Populations: Human Evolution and Complex Disease"* |
|
1128 |
+**DOI:** [10.1038/nrg865](https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg865) |
|
1129 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Population Genetics, Human Evolution, Complex Diseases* |
1536 |
1536 |
|
1537 |
|
- |
1538 |
|
- |
1539 |
|
-= Whiteness = |
1540 |
|
- |
1541 |
|
-== Study: Segregation, Innocence, and Protection: The Institutional Conditions That Maintain Whiteness in College Sports == |
1542 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Segregation, Innocence, and Protection: The Institutional Conditions That Maintain Whiteness in College Sports" expanded="false"}} |
1543 |
|
-**Source:** *Journal of Diversity in Higher Education* |
1544 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2019* |
1545 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Kirsten Hextrum* |
1546 |
|
-**Title:** *"Segregation, Innocence, and Protection: The Institutional Conditions That Maintain Whiteness in College Sports"* |
1547 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1037/dhe0000140](https://doi.org/10.1037/dhe0000140) |
1548 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Race and Sports, Higher Education, Institutional Racism* |
1549 |
|
- |
1550 |
1550 |
--- |
1551 |
1551 |
|
1552 |
1552 |
## **Key Statistics** |
1553 |
1553 |
1. **General Observations:** |
1554 |
|
- - Analyzed **47 college athlete narratives** to explore racial disparities in non-revenue sports. |
1555 |
|
- - Found three interrelated themes: **racial segregation, racial innocence, and racial protection**. |
|
1135 |
+ - Africa harbors **the highest genetic diversity** of any region, making it key to understanding human evolution. |
|
1136 |
+ - The study analyzes **genetic variation and linkage disequilibrium (LD) in African populations**. |
1556 |
1556 |
|
1557 |
1557 |
2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1558 |
|
- - **Predominantly white sports programs** reinforce racial hierarchies in college athletics. |
1559 |
|
- - **Recruitment policies favor white athletes** from affluent, suburban backgrounds. |
|
1139 |
+ - African populations exhibit **greater genetic differentiation compared to non-Africans**. |
|
1140 |
+ - **Migration and admixture** have shaped modern African genomes over the past **100,000 years**. |
1560 |
1560 |
|
1561 |
1561 |
3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1562 |
|
- - White athletes are **socialized to remain unaware of racial privilege** in their athletic careers. |
1563 |
|
- - Media and institutional narratives protect white athletes from discussions on race and systemic inequities. |
|
1143 |
+ - The **effective population size (Ne) of Africans** is higher than that of non-African populations. |
|
1144 |
+ - LD blocks are **shorter in African genomes**, suggesting more historical recombination events. |
1564 |
1564 |
|
1565 |
1565 |
--- |
1566 |
1566 |
|
1567 |
1567 |
## **Findings** |
1568 |
1568 |
1. **Primary Observations:** |
1569 |
|
- - Colleges **actively recruit white athletes** from majority-white communities. |
1570 |
|
- - Institutional policies **uphold whiteness** by failing to challenge racial biases in recruitment and team culture. |
|
1150 |
+ - African populations are the **most genetically diverse**, supporting the *Recent African Origin* hypothesis. |
|
1151 |
+ - Genetic variation in African populations can **help fine-map complex disease genes**. |
1571 |
1571 |
|
1572 |
1572 |
2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1573 |
|
- - **White athletes show limited awareness** of their racial advantage in sports. |
1574 |
|
- - **Black athletes are overrepresented** in revenue-generating sports but underrepresented in non-revenue teams. |
|
1154 |
+ - **West Africans exhibit higher genetic diversity** than East Africans due to differing migration patterns. |
|
1155 |
+ - Populations such as **San hunter-gatherers show deep genetic divergence**. |
1575 |
1575 |
|
1576 |
1576 |
3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1577 |
|
- - Examines **how sports serve as a mechanism for maintaining racial privilege** in higher education. |
1578 |
|
- - Discusses the **role of athletics in reinforcing systemic segregation and exclusion**. |
|
1158 |
+ - Admixture in African Americans includes **West African and European genetic contributions**. |
|
1159 |
+ - SNP (single nucleotide polymorphism) diversity in African genomes **exceeds that of non-African groups**. |
1579 |
1579 |
|
1580 |
1580 |
--- |
1581 |
1581 |
|
1582 |
1582 |
## **Critique and Observations** |
1583 |
1583 |
1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1584 |
|
- - **Comprehensive qualitative analysis** of race in college sports. |
1585 |
|
- - Examines **institutional conditions** that sustain racial disparities in athletics. |
|
1165 |
+ - Provides **comprehensive genetic analysis** of diverse African populations. |
|
1166 |
+ - Highlights **how genetic diversity impacts health disparities and disease risks**. |
1586 |
1586 |
|
1587 |
1587 |
2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1588 |
|
- - Focuses primarily on **Division I non-revenue sports**, limiting generalizability to other divisions. |
1589 |
|
- - Lacks extensive **quantitative data on racial demographics** in college athletics. |
|
1169 |
+ - Many **African populations remain understudied**, limiting full understanding of diversity. |
|
1170 |
+ - Focuses more on genetic variation than on **specific disease mechanisms**. |
1590 |
1590 |
|
1591 |
1591 |
3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1592 |
|
- - Future research should **compare recruitment policies across different sports and divisions**. |
1593 |
|
- - Investigate **how athletic scholarships contribute to racial inequities in higher education**. |
|
1173 |
+ - Expand research into **underrepresented African populations**. |
|
1174 |
+ - Integrate **whole-genome sequencing for a more detailed evolutionary timeline**. |
1594 |
1594 |
|
1595 |
1595 |
--- |
1596 |
1596 |
|
1597 |
1597 |
## **Relevance to Subproject** |
1598 |
|
-- Provides evidence of **systemic racial biases** in college sports recruitment. |
1599 |
|
-- Highlights **how institutional policies protect whiteness** in non-revenue athletics. |
1600 |
|
-- Supports research on **diversity, equity, and inclusion (DEI) efforts in sports and education**. |
|
1179 |
+- Supports **genetic models of human evolution** and the **out-of-Africa hypothesis**. |
|
1180 |
+- Reinforces **Africa’s key role in disease gene mapping and precision medicine**. |
|
1181 |
+- Provides insight into **historical migration patterns and their genetic impact**. |
1601 |
1601 |
|
1602 |
1602 |
--- |
1603 |
1603 |
|
1604 |
1604 |
## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
1605 |
|
-1. Investigate how **racial stereotypes influence college athlete recruitment**. |
1606 |
|
-2. Examine **the role of media in shaping public perceptions of race in sports**. |
1607 |
|
-3. Explore **policy reforms to increase racial diversity in non-revenue sports**. |
|
1186 |
+1. Investigate **genetic adaptations to local environments within Africa**. |
|
1187 |
+2. Study **the role of African genetic diversity in disease resistance**. |
|
1188 |
+3. Expand research on **how ancient migration patterns shaped modern genetic structure**. |
1608 |
1608 |
|
1609 |
1609 |
--- |
1610 |
1610 |
|
1611 |
1611 |
## **Summary of Research Study** |
1612 |
|
-This study explores how **racial segregation, innocence, and protection** sustain whiteness in college sports. By analyzing **47 athlete narratives**, the research reveals **how predominantly white sports programs recruit and retain white athletes** while shielding them from discussions on race. The findings highlight **institutional biases that maintain racial privilege in athletics**, offering critical insight into the **structural inequalities in higher education sports programs**. |
|
1193 |
+This study explores the **genetic diversity of African populations**, analyzing their role in **human evolution and complex disease research**. The findings highlight **Africa’s unique genetic landscape**, confirming it as the most genetically diverse continent. The research provides valuable insights into **how genetic variation influences disease susceptibility, evolution, and population structure**. |
1613 |
1613 |
|
1614 |
1614 |
This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
1615 |
1615 |
|
... |
... |
@@ -1616,17 +1616,14 @@ |
1616 |
1616 |
--- |
1617 |
1617 |
|
1618 |
1618 |
## **📄 Download Full Study** |
1619 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1037_dhe0000140.pdf]] |
|
1200 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nrg865MODERN.pdf]] |
1620 |
1620 |
|
1621 |
1621 |
{{/expand}} |
1622 |
1622 |
|
|
1204 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
1623 |
1623 |
|
1624 |
1624 |
|
1625 |
1625 |
|
1626 |
|
- |
1627 |
|
-= White Guilt = |
1628 |
|
- |
1629 |
|
-== Study: Racial Bias in Pain Assessment and Treatment Recommendations == |
1630 |
1630 |
{{expand title="Study: Racial Bias in Pain Assessment and Treatment Recommendations" expanded="false"}} |
1631 |
1631 |
**Source:** *Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS)* |
1632 |
1632 |
**Date of Publication:** *2016* |
... |
... |
@@ -1708,8 +1708,9 @@ |
1708 |
1708 |
|
1709 |
1709 |
{{/expand}} |
1710 |
1710 |
|
|
1289 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
1711 |
1711 |
|
1712 |
|
-== Study: Rising Morbidity and Mortality in Midlife Among White Non-Hispanic Americans == |
|
1291 |
+ |
1713 |
1713 |
{{expand title="Study: Rising Morbidity and Mortality in Midlife Among White Non-Hispanic Americans" expanded="false"}} |
1714 |
1714 |
**Source:** *Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS)* |
1715 |
1715 |
**Date of Publication:** *2015* |
... |
... |
@@ -1791,7 +1791,8 @@ |
1791 |
1791 |
|
1792 |
1792 |
{{/expand}} |
1793 |
1793 |
|
1794 |
|
-== Study: How Do People Without Migration Background Experience and Impact Today’s Superdiverse Cities? == |
|
1373 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
|
1374 |
+ |
1795 |
1795 |
{{expand title="Study: How Do People Without Migration Background Experience and Impact Today’s Superdiverse Cities?" expanded="false"}} |
1796 |
1796 |
**Source:** *Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies* |
1797 |
1797 |
**Date of Publication:** *2023* |
... |
... |
@@ -1873,250 +1873,6 @@ |
1873 |
1873 |
|
1874 |
1874 |
{{/expand}} |
1875 |
1875 |
|
|
1456 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
1876 |
1876 |
|
1877 |
1877 |
|
1878 |
|
-= Media = |
1879 |
|
- |
1880 |
|
-== Study: The Role of Computer-Mediated Communication in Intergroup Conflic == |
1881 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: The Role of Computer-Mediated Communication in Intergroup Conflict" expanded="false"}} |
1882 |
|
-**Source:** *Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication* |
1883 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2021* |
1884 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Zeynep Tufekci, Jesse Fox, Andrew Chadwick* |
1885 |
|
-**Title:** *"The Role of Computer-Mediated Communication in Intergroup Conflict"* |
1886 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1093/jcmc/zmab003](https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmab003) |
1887 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Online Communication, Social Media, Conflict Studies* |
1888 |
|
- |
1889 |
|
---- |
1890 |
|
- |
1891 |
|
-## **Key Statistics** |
1892 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1893 |
|
- - Analyzed **over 500,000 social media interactions** related to intergroup conflict. |
1894 |
|
- - Found that **computer-mediated communication (CMC) intensifies polarization**. |
1895 |
|
- |
1896 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1897 |
|
- - **Anonymity and reduced social cues** in CMC increased hostility. |
1898 |
|
- - **Echo chambers formed more frequently in algorithm-driven environments**. |
1899 |
|
- |
1900 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1901 |
|
- - **Misinformation spread 3x faster** in polarized online discussions. |
1902 |
|
- - Users exposed to **conflicting viewpoints were more likely to engage in retaliatory discourse**. |
1903 |
|
- |
1904 |
|
---- |
1905 |
|
- |
1906 |
|
-## **Findings** |
1907 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1908 |
|
- - **Online interactions amplify intergroup conflict** due to selective exposure and confirmation bias. |
1909 |
|
- - **Algorithmic sorting contributes to ideological segmentation**. |
1910 |
|
- |
1911 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1912 |
|
- - Participants with **strong pre-existing biases became more polarized** after exposure to conflicting views. |
1913 |
|
- - **Moderate users were more likely to disengage** from conflict-heavy discussions. |
1914 |
|
- |
1915 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1916 |
|
- - **CMC increased political tribalism** in digital spaces. |
1917 |
|
- - **Emotional language spread more widely** than factual content. |
1918 |
|
- |
1919 |
|
---- |
1920 |
|
- |
1921 |
|
-## **Critique and Observations** |
1922 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1923 |
|
- - **Largest dataset** to date analyzing **CMC and intergroup conflict**. |
1924 |
|
- - Uses **longitudinal data tracking user behavior over time**. |
1925 |
|
- |
1926 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1927 |
|
- - Lacks **qualitative analysis of user motivations**. |
1928 |
|
- - Focuses on **Western social media platforms**, missing global perspectives. |
1929 |
|
- |
1930 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1931 |
|
- - Future studies should **analyze private messaging platforms** in conflict dynamics. |
1932 |
|
- - Investigate **interventions that reduce online polarization**. |
1933 |
|
- |
1934 |
|
---- |
1935 |
|
- |
1936 |
|
-## **Relevance to Subproject** |
1937 |
|
-- Explores how **digital communication influences social division**. |
1938 |
|
-- Supports research on **social media regulation and conflict mitigation**. |
1939 |
|
-- Provides **data on misinformation and online radicalization trends**. |
1940 |
|
- |
1941 |
|
---- |
1942 |
|
- |
1943 |
|
-## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
1944 |
|
-1. Investigate **how online anonymity affects real-world aggression**. |
1945 |
|
-2. Study **social media interventions that reduce political polarization**. |
1946 |
|
-3. Explore **cross-cultural differences in CMC and intergroup hostility**. |
1947 |
|
- |
1948 |
|
---- |
1949 |
|
- |
1950 |
|
-## **Summary of Research Study** |
1951 |
|
-This study examines **how online communication intensifies intergroup conflict**, using a dataset of **500,000+ social media interactions**. It highlights the role of **algorithmic filtering, anonymity, and selective exposure** in **increasing polarization and misinformation spread**. The findings emphasize the **need for policy interventions to mitigate digital conflict escalation**. |
1952 |
|
- |
1953 |
|
---- |
1954 |
|
- |
1955 |
|
-## **📄 Download Full Study** |
1956 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1093_jcmc_zmab003.pdf]] |
1957 |
|
- |
1958 |
|
-{{/expand}} |
1959 |
|
- |
1960 |
|
- |
1961 |
|
-== Study: Equality, Morality, and the Impact of Media Framing on Same-Sex Marriage and Civil Unions == |
1962 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: Equality, Morality, and the Impact of Media Framing on Same-Sex Marriage and Civil Unions" expanded="false"}} |
1963 |
|
-**Source:** *Politics & Policy* |
1964 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2007* |
1965 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Tyler Johnson* |
1966 |
|
-**Title:** *"Equality, Morality, and the Impact of Media Framing: Explaining Opposition to Same-Sex Marriage and Civil Unions"* |
1967 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1111/j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x](https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x) |
1968 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *LGBTQ+ Rights, Public Opinion, Media Influence* |
1969 |
|
- |
1970 |
|
---- |
1971 |
|
- |
1972 |
|
-## **Key Statistics** |
1973 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1974 |
|
- - Examines **media coverage of same-sex marriage and civil unions from 2004 to 2011**. |
1975 |
|
- - Analyzes how **media framing influences public opinion trends** on LGBTQ+ rights. |
1976 |
|
- |
1977 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1978 |
|
- - **Equality-based framing decreases opposition** to same-sex marriage. |
1979 |
|
- - **Morality-based framing increases opposition** to same-sex marriage. |
1980 |
|
- |
1981 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1982 |
|
- - When **equality framing surpasses morality framing**, public opposition declines. |
1983 |
|
- - Media framing **directly affects public attitudes** over time, shaping policy debates. |
1984 |
|
- |
1985 |
|
---- |
1986 |
|
- |
1987 |
|
-## **Findings** |
1988 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1989 |
|
- - **Media framing plays a critical role in shaping attitudes** toward LGBTQ+ rights. |
1990 |
|
- - **Equality-focused narratives** lead to greater public support for same-sex marriage. |
1991 |
|
- |
1992 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1993 |
|
- - **Religious and conservative audiences** respond more to morality-based framing. |
1994 |
|
- - **Younger and progressive audiences** respond more to equality-based framing. |
1995 |
|
- |
1996 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1997 |
|
- - **Periods of increased equality framing** saw measurable **declines in opposition to LGBTQ+ rights**. |
1998 |
|
- - **Major political events (elections, Supreme Court cases) influenced framing trends**. |
1999 |
|
- |
2000 |
|
---- |
2001 |
|
- |
2002 |
|
-## **Critique and Observations** |
2003 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
2004 |
|
- - **Longitudinal dataset spanning multiple election cycles**. |
2005 |
|
- - Provides **quantitative analysis of how media framing shifts public opinion**. |
2006 |
|
- |
2007 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
2008 |
|
- - Focuses **only on U.S. media coverage**, limiting global applicability. |
2009 |
|
- - Does not account for **social media's growing influence** on public opinion. |
2010 |
|
- |
2011 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
2012 |
|
- - Expand the study to **global perspectives on LGBTQ+ rights and media influence**. |
2013 |
|
- - Investigate how **different media platforms (TV vs. digital media) impact opinion shifts**. |
2014 |
|
- |
2015 |
|
---- |
2016 |
|
- |
2017 |
|
-## **Relevance to Subproject** |
2018 |
|
-- Explores **how media narratives shape policy support and public sentiment**. |
2019 |
|
-- Highlights **the strategic importance of framing in LGBTQ+ advocacy**. |
2020 |
|
-- Reinforces the need for **media literacy in understanding policy debates**. |
2021 |
|
- |
2022 |
|
---- |
2023 |
|
- |
2024 |
|
-## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
2025 |
|
-1. Examine how **social media affects framing of LGBTQ+ issues**. |
2026 |
|
-2. Study **differences in framing across political media outlets**. |
2027 |
|
-3. Investigate **public opinion shifts in states that legalized same-sex marriage earlier**. |
2028 |
|
- |
2029 |
|
---- |
2030 |
|
- |
2031 |
|
-## **Summary of Research Study** |
2032 |
|
-This study examines **how media framing influences public attitudes on same-sex marriage and civil unions**, analyzing **news coverage from 2004 to 2011**. It finds that **equality-based narratives reduce opposition, while morality-based narratives increase it**. The research highlights **how media coverage plays a crucial role in shaping policy debates and public sentiment**. |
2033 |
|
- |
2034 |
|
---- |
2035 |
|
- |
2036 |
|
-## **📄 Download Full Study** |
2037 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1111_j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x_abstract.pdf]] |
2038 |
|
- |
2039 |
|
-{{/expand}} |
2040 |
|
- |
2041 |
|
-== Study: The Effects of Digital Media on Political Persuasion == |
2042 |
|
-{{expand title="Study: The Effects of Digital Media on Political Persuasion" expanded="false"}} |
2043 |
|
-**Source:** *Journal of Communication* |
2044 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2019* |
2045 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Natalie Stroud, Matthew Barnidge, Shannon McGregor* |
2046 |
|
-**Title:** *"The Effects of Digital Media on Political Persuasion: Evidence from Experimental Studies"* |
2047 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1093/joc/jqx021](https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqx021) |
2048 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Media Influence, Political Communication, Persuasion* |
2049 |
|
- |
2050 |
|
---- |
2051 |
|
- |
2052 |
|
-## **Key Statistics** |
2053 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
2054 |
|
- - Conducted **12 experimental studies** on **digital media's impact on political beliefs**. |
2055 |
|
- - **58% of participants** showed shifts in political opinion based on online content. |
2056 |
|
- |
2057 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
2058 |
|
- - **Video-based content was 2x more persuasive** than text-based content. |
2059 |
|
- - Participants **under age 35 were more susceptible to political messaging shifts**. |
2060 |
|
- |
2061 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
2062 |
|
- - **Interactive media (comment sections, polls) increased political engagement**. |
2063 |
|
- - **Exposure to counterarguments reduced partisan bias** by **14% on average**. |
2064 |
|
- |
2065 |
|
---- |
2066 |
|
- |
2067 |
|
-## **Findings** |
2068 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
2069 |
|
- - **Digital media significantly influences political opinions**, with younger audiences being the most impacted. |
2070 |
|
- - **Multimedia content is more persuasive** than traditional text-based arguments. |
2071 |
|
- |
2072 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
2073 |
|
- - **Social media platforms had stronger persuasive effects** than news websites. |
2074 |
|
- - Participants who engaged in **online discussions retained more political knowledge**. |
2075 |
|
- |
2076 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
2077 |
|
- - **Highly partisan users became more entrenched in their views**, even when exposed to opposing content. |
2078 |
|
- - **Neutral or apolitical users were more likely to shift opinions**. |
2079 |
|
- |
2080 |
|
---- |
2081 |
|
- |
2082 |
|
-## **Critique and Observations** |
2083 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
2084 |
|
- - **Large-scale experimental design** allows for controlled comparisons. |
2085 |
|
- - Covers **multiple digital platforms**, ensuring robust findings. |
2086 |
|
- |
2087 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
2088 |
|
- - Limited to **short-term persuasion effects**, without long-term follow-up. |
2089 |
|
- - Does not explore **the role of misinformation in political persuasion**. |
2090 |
|
- |
2091 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
2092 |
|
- - Future studies should track **long-term opinion changes** beyond immediate reactions. |
2093 |
|
- - Investigate **the role of digital media literacy in resisting persuasion**. |
2094 |
|
- |
2095 |
|
---- |
2096 |
|
- |
2097 |
|
-## **Relevance to Subproject** |
2098 |
|
-- Provides insights into **how digital media shapes political discourse**. |
2099 |
|
-- Highlights **which platforms and content types are most influential**. |
2100 |
|
-- Supports **research on misinformation and online political engagement**. |
2101 |
|
- |
2102 |
|
---- |
2103 |
|
- |
2104 |
|
-## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
2105 |
|
-1. Study how **fact-checking influences digital persuasion effects**. |
2106 |
|
-2. Investigate the **role of political influencers in shaping opinions**. |
2107 |
|
-3. Explore **long-term effects of social media exposure on political beliefs**. |
2108 |
|
- |
2109 |
|
---- |
2110 |
|
- |
2111 |
|
-## **Summary of Research Study** |
2112 |
|
-This study analyzes **how digital media influences political persuasion**, using **12 experimental studies**. The findings show that **video and interactive content are the most persuasive**, while **younger users are more susceptible to political messaging shifts**. The research emphasizes the **power of digital platforms in shaping public opinion and engagement**. |
2113 |
|
- |
2114 |
|
---- |
2115 |
|
- |
2116 |
|
-## **📄 Download Full Study** |
2117 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1093_joc_jqx021.pdf]] |
2118 |
|
- |
2119 |
|
-{{/expand}} |
2120 |
|
- |
2121 |
|
- |
2122 |
|
- |