... |
... |
@@ -1455,4 +1455,745 @@ |
1455 |
1455 |
|
1456 |
1456 |
{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
1457 |
1457 |
|
|
1458 |
+{{expand title="Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program" expanded="false"}} |
|
1459 |
+**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
|
1460 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2002* |
|
1461 |
+**Author(s):** *Clifford A. Butzin, Christine A. Saum, Frank R. Scarpitti* |
|
1462 |
+**Title:** *"Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"* |
|
1463 |
+**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120014424](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120014424) |
|
1464 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Substance Use, Criminal Justice, Drug Courts* |
1458 |
1458 |
|
|
1466 |
+--- |
|
1467 |
+ |
|
1468 |
+## **Key Statistics** |
|
1469 |
+1. **General Observations:** |
|
1470 |
+ - Study examined **drug treatment court success rates** among first-time offenders. |
|
1471 |
+ - Strongest predictors of **successful completion were employment status and race**. |
|
1472 |
+ |
|
1473 |
+2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
|
1474 |
+ - Individuals with **stable jobs were more likely to complete the program**. |
|
1475 |
+ - **Black participants had lower success rates**, suggesting potential systemic disparities. |
|
1476 |
+ |
|
1477 |
+3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
|
1478 |
+ - **Education level was positively correlated** with program completion. |
|
1479 |
+ - Frequency of **drug use before enrollment affected treatment outcomes**. |
|
1480 |
+ |
|
1481 |
+--- |
|
1482 |
+ |
|
1483 |
+## **Findings** |
|
1484 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
1485 |
+ - **Social stability factors** (employment, education) were key to treatment success. |
|
1486 |
+ - **Race and pre-existing substance use patterns** influenced completion rates. |
|
1487 |
+ |
|
1488 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
1489 |
+ - White offenders had **higher completion rates** than Black offenders. |
|
1490 |
+ - Drug court success was **higher for those with lower initial drug use frequency**. |
|
1491 |
+ |
|
1492 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
1493 |
+ - **Individuals with strong social ties were more likely to finish the program**. |
|
1494 |
+ - Success rates were **significantly higher for participants with case management support**. |
|
1495 |
+ |
|
1496 |
+--- |
|
1497 |
+ |
|
1498 |
+## **Critique and Observations** |
|
1499 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
1500 |
+ - **First empirical study on drug court program success factors**. |
|
1501 |
+ - Uses **longitudinal data** for post-treatment analysis. |
|
1502 |
+ |
|
1503 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
1504 |
+ - Lacks **qualitative data on personal motivation and treatment engagement**. |
|
1505 |
+ - Focuses on **short-term program success** without tracking **long-term relapse rates**. |
|
1506 |
+ |
|
1507 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
1508 |
+ - Future research should examine **racial disparities in drug court outcomes**. |
|
1509 |
+ - Study **how community resources impact long-term recovery**. |
|
1510 |
+ |
|
1511 |
+--- |
|
1512 |
+ |
|
1513 |
+## **Relevance to Subproject** |
|
1514 |
+- Provides insight into **what factors contribute to drug court program success**. |
|
1515 |
+- Highlights **racial disparities in criminal justice-based rehabilitation programs**. |
|
1516 |
+- Supports **policy discussions on improving access to drug treatment for marginalized groups**. |
|
1517 |
+ |
|
1518 |
+--- |
|
1519 |
+ |
|
1520 |
+## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
|
1521 |
+1. Investigate **the role of mental health in drug court success rates**. |
|
1522 |
+2. Assess **long-term relapse prevention strategies post-treatment**. |
|
1523 |
+3. Explore **alternative diversion programs beyond traditional drug courts**. |
|
1524 |
+ |
|
1525 |
+--- |
|
1526 |
+ |
|
1527 |
+## **Summary of Research Study** |
|
1528 |
+This study examines **factors influencing the completion of drug treatment court programs**, identifying **employment, education, and race as key predictors**. The research underscores **systemic disparities in drug court outcomes**, emphasizing the need for **improved support systems for at-risk populations**. |
|
1529 |
+ |
|
1530 |
+This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
|
1531 |
+ |
|
1532 |
+--- |
|
1533 |
+ |
|
1534 |
+## **📄 Download Full Study** |
|
1535 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120014424.pdf]] |
|
1536 |
+ |
|
1537 |
+{{/expand}} |
|
1538 |
+ |
|
1539 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
|
1540 |
+ |
|
1541 |
+ |
|
1542 |
+{{expand title="Study: Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys" expanded="false"}} |
|
1543 |
+**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
|
1544 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2003* |
|
1545 |
+**Author(s):** *Timothy P. Johnson, Phillip J. Bowman* |
|
1546 |
+**Title:** *"Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys"* |
|
1547 |
+**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120023394](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120023394) |
|
1548 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Survey Methodology, Racial Disparities, Substance Use Research* |
|
1549 |
+ |
|
1550 |
+--- |
|
1551 |
+ |
|
1552 |
+## **Key Statistics** |
|
1553 |
+1. **General Observations:** |
|
1554 |
+ - Study examined **how racial and cultural factors influence self-reported substance use data**. |
|
1555 |
+ - Analyzed **36 empirical studies from 1977–2003** on survey reliability across racial/ethnic groups. |
|
1556 |
+ |
|
1557 |
+2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
|
1558 |
+ - Black and Latino respondents **were more likely to underreport drug use** compared to White respondents. |
|
1559 |
+ - **Cultural stigma and distrust in research institutions** affected self-report accuracy. |
|
1560 |
+ |
|
1561 |
+3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
|
1562 |
+ - **Surveys using biological validation (urinalysis, hair tests) revealed underreporting trends**. |
|
1563 |
+ - **Higher recantation rates** (denying past drug use) were observed among minority respondents. |
|
1564 |
+ |
|
1565 |
+--- |
|
1566 |
+ |
|
1567 |
+## **Findings** |
|
1568 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
1569 |
+ - Racial/ethnic disparities in **substance use reporting bias survey-based research**. |
|
1570 |
+ - **Social desirability and cultural norms impact data reliability**. |
|
1571 |
+ |
|
1572 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
1573 |
+ - White respondents were **more likely to overreport** substance use. |
|
1574 |
+ - Black and Latino respondents **had higher recantation rates**, particularly in face-to-face interviews. |
|
1575 |
+ |
|
1576 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
1577 |
+ - Mode of survey administration **significantly influenced reporting accuracy**. |
|
1578 |
+ - **Self-administered surveys produced more reliable data than interviewer-administered surveys**. |
|
1579 |
+ |
|
1580 |
+--- |
|
1581 |
+ |
|
1582 |
+## **Critique and Observations** |
|
1583 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
1584 |
+ - **Comprehensive review of 36 studies** on measurement error in substance use reporting. |
|
1585 |
+ - Identifies **systemic biases affecting racial/ethnic survey reliability**. |
|
1586 |
+ |
|
1587 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
1588 |
+ - Relies on **secondary data analysis**, limiting direct experimental control. |
|
1589 |
+ - Does not explore **how measurement error impacts policy decisions**. |
|
1590 |
+ |
|
1591 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
1592 |
+ - Future research should **incorporate mixed-method approaches** (qualitative & quantitative). |
|
1593 |
+ - Investigate **how survey design can reduce racial reporting disparities**. |
|
1594 |
+ |
|
1595 |
+--- |
|
1596 |
+ |
|
1597 |
+## **Relevance to Subproject** |
|
1598 |
+- Supports research on **racial disparities in self-reported health behaviors**. |
|
1599 |
+- Highlights **survey methodology issues that impact substance use epidemiology**. |
|
1600 |
+- Provides insights for **improving data accuracy in public health research**. |
|
1601 |
+ |
|
1602 |
+--- |
|
1603 |
+ |
|
1604 |
+## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
|
1605 |
+1. Investigate **how survey design impacts racial disparities in self-reported health data**. |
|
1606 |
+2. Study **alternative data collection methods (biometric validation, passive data tracking)**. |
|
1607 |
+3. Explore **the role of social stigma in self-reported health behaviors**. |
|
1608 |
+ |
|
1609 |
+--- |
|
1610 |
+ |
|
1611 |
+## **Summary of Research Study** |
|
1612 |
+This study examines **cross-cultural biases in self-reported substance use surveys**, showing that **racial/ethnic minorities are more likely to underreport drug use** due to **social stigma, research distrust, and survey administration methods**. The findings highlight **critical issues in public health data collection and the need for improved survey design**. |
|
1613 |
+ |
|
1614 |
+This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
|
1615 |
+ |
|
1616 |
+--- |
|
1617 |
+ |
|
1618 |
+## **📄 Download Full Study** |
|
1619 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120023394.pdf]] |
|
1620 |
+ |
|
1621 |
+{{/expand}} |
|
1622 |
+ |
|
1623 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
|
1624 |
+ |
|
1625 |
+{{expand title="Study: Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys" expanded="false"}} |
|
1626 |
+**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
|
1627 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2003* |
|
1628 |
+**Author(s):** *Timothy P. Johnson, Phillip J. Bowman* |
|
1629 |
+**Title:** *"Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys"* |
|
1630 |
+**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120023394](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120023394) |
|
1631 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Survey Methodology, Racial Disparities, Substance Use Research* |
|
1632 |
+ |
|
1633 |
+--- |
|
1634 |
+ |
|
1635 |
+## **Key Statistics** |
|
1636 |
+1. **General Observations:** |
|
1637 |
+ - Study examined **how racial and cultural factors influence self-reported substance use data**. |
|
1638 |
+ - Analyzed **36 empirical studies from 1977–2003** on survey reliability across racial/ethnic groups. |
|
1639 |
+ |
|
1640 |
+2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
|
1641 |
+ - Black and Latino respondents **were more likely to underreport drug use** compared to White respondents. |
|
1642 |
+ - **Cultural stigma and distrust in research institutions** affected self-report accuracy. |
|
1643 |
+ |
|
1644 |
+3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
|
1645 |
+ - **Surveys using biological validation (urinalysis, hair tests) revealed underreporting trends**. |
|
1646 |
+ - **Higher recantation rates** (denying past drug use) were observed among minority respondents. |
|
1647 |
+ |
|
1648 |
+--- |
|
1649 |
+ |
|
1650 |
+## **Findings** |
|
1651 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
1652 |
+ - Racial/ethnic disparities in **substance use reporting bias survey-based research**. |
|
1653 |
+ - **Social desirability and cultural norms impact data reliability**. |
|
1654 |
+ |
|
1655 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
1656 |
+ - White respondents were **more likely to overreport** substance use. |
|
1657 |
+ - Black and Latino respondents **had higher recantation rates**, particularly in face-to-face interviews. |
|
1658 |
+ |
|
1659 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
1660 |
+ - Mode of survey administration **significantly influenced reporting accuracy**. |
|
1661 |
+ - **Self-administered surveys produced more reliable data than interviewer-administered surveys**. |
|
1662 |
+ |
|
1663 |
+--- |
|
1664 |
+ |
|
1665 |
+## **Critique and Observations** |
|
1666 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
1667 |
+ - **Comprehensive review of 36 studies** on measurement error in substance use reporting. |
|
1668 |
+ - Identifies **systemic biases affecting racial/ethnic survey reliability**. |
|
1669 |
+ |
|
1670 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
1671 |
+ - Relies on **secondary data analysis**, limiting direct experimental control. |
|
1672 |
+ - Does not explore **how measurement error impacts policy decisions**. |
|
1673 |
+ |
|
1674 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
1675 |
+ - Future research should **incorporate mixed-method approaches** (qualitative & quantitative). |
|
1676 |
+ - Investigate **how survey design can reduce racial reporting disparities**. |
|
1677 |
+ |
|
1678 |
+--- |
|
1679 |
+ |
|
1680 |
+## **Relevance to Subproject** |
|
1681 |
+- Supports research on **racial disparities in self-reported health behaviors**. |
|
1682 |
+- Highlights **survey methodology issues that impact substance use epidemiology**. |
|
1683 |
+- Provides insights for **improving data accuracy in public health research**. |
|
1684 |
+ |
|
1685 |
+--- |
|
1686 |
+ |
|
1687 |
+## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
|
1688 |
+1. Investigate **how survey design impacts racial disparities in self-reported health data**. |
|
1689 |
+2. Study **alternative data collection methods (biometric validation, passive data tracking)**. |
|
1690 |
+3. Explore **the role of social stigma in self-reported health behaviors**. |
|
1691 |
+ |
|
1692 |
+--- |
|
1693 |
+ |
|
1694 |
+## **Summary of Research Study** |
|
1695 |
+This study examines **cross-cultural biases in self-reported substance use surveys**, showing that **racial/ethnic minorities are more likely to underreport drug use** due to **social stigma, research distrust, and survey administration methods**. The findings highlight **critical issues in public health data collection and the need for improved survey design**. |
|
1696 |
+ |
|
1697 |
+This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
|
1698 |
+ |
|
1699 |
+--- |
|
1700 |
+ |
|
1701 |
+## **📄 Download Full Study** |
|
1702 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120023394.pdf]] |
|
1703 |
+ |
|
1704 |
+{{/expand}} |
|
1705 |
+ |
|
1706 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
|
1707 |
+ |
|
1708 |
+{{expand title="Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program" expanded="false"}} |
|
1709 |
+**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
|
1710 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2002* |
|
1711 |
+**Author(s):** *Clifford A. Butzin, Christine A. Saum, Frank R. Scarpitti* |
|
1712 |
+**Title:** *"Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"* |
|
1713 |
+**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120014424](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120014424) |
|
1714 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Substance Use, Criminal Justice, Drug Courts* |
|
1715 |
+ |
|
1716 |
+--- |
|
1717 |
+ |
|
1718 |
+## **Key Statistics** |
|
1719 |
+1. **General Observations:** |
|
1720 |
+ - Study examined **drug treatment court success rates** among first-time offenders. |
|
1721 |
+ - Strongest predictors of **successful completion were employment status and race**. |
|
1722 |
+ |
|
1723 |
+2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
|
1724 |
+ - Individuals with **stable jobs were more likely to complete the program**. |
|
1725 |
+ - **Black participants had lower success rates**, suggesting potential systemic disparities. |
|
1726 |
+ |
|
1727 |
+3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
|
1728 |
+ - **Education level was positively correlated** with program completion. |
|
1729 |
+ - Frequency of **drug use before enrollment affected treatment outcomes**. |
|
1730 |
+ |
|
1731 |
+--- |
|
1732 |
+ |
|
1733 |
+## **Findings** |
|
1734 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
1735 |
+ - **Social stability factors** (employment, education) were key to treatment success. |
|
1736 |
+ - **Race and pre-existing substance use patterns** influenced completion rates. |
|
1737 |
+ |
|
1738 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
1739 |
+ - White offenders had **higher completion rates** than Black offenders. |
|
1740 |
+ - Drug court success was **higher for those with lower initial drug use frequency**. |
|
1741 |
+ |
|
1742 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
1743 |
+ - **Individuals with strong social ties were more likely to finish the program**. |
|
1744 |
+ - Success rates were **significantly higher for participants with case management support**. |
|
1745 |
+ |
|
1746 |
+--- |
|
1747 |
+ |
|
1748 |
+## **Critique and Observations** |
|
1749 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
1750 |
+ - **First empirical study on drug court program success factors**. |
|
1751 |
+ - Uses **longitudinal data** for post-treatment analysis. |
|
1752 |
+ |
|
1753 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
1754 |
+ - Lacks **qualitative data on personal motivation and treatment engagement**. |
|
1755 |
+ - Focuses on **short-term program success** without tracking **long-term relapse rates**. |
|
1756 |
+ |
|
1757 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
1758 |
+ - Future research should examine **racial disparities in drug court outcomes**. |
|
1759 |
+ - Study **how community resources impact long-term recovery**. |
|
1760 |
+ |
|
1761 |
+--- |
|
1762 |
+ |
|
1763 |
+## **Relevance to Subproject** |
|
1764 |
+- Provides insight into **what factors contribute to drug court program success**. |
|
1765 |
+- Highlights **racial disparities in criminal justice-based rehabilitation programs**. |
|
1766 |
+- Supports **policy discussions on improving access to drug treatment for marginalized groups**. |
|
1767 |
+ |
|
1768 |
+--- |
|
1769 |
+ |
|
1770 |
+## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
|
1771 |
+1. Investigate **the role of mental health in drug court success rates**. |
|
1772 |
+2. Assess **long-term relapse prevention strategies post-treatment**. |
|
1773 |
+3. Explore **alternative diversion programs beyond traditional drug courts**. |
|
1774 |
+ |
|
1775 |
+--- |
|
1776 |
+ |
|
1777 |
+## **Summary of Research Study** |
|
1778 |
+This study examines **factors influencing the completion of drug treatment court programs**, identifying **employment, education, and race as key predictors**. The research underscores **systemic disparities in drug court outcomes**, emphasizing the need for **improved support systems for at-risk populations**. |
|
1779 |
+ |
|
1780 |
+This summary provides an accessible, at-a-glance overview of the study’s contributions. Please refer to the full paper for in-depth analysis. |
|
1781 |
+ |
|
1782 |
+--- |
|
1783 |
+ |
|
1784 |
+## **📄 Download Full Study** |
|
1785 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120014424.pdf]] |
|
1786 |
+ |
|
1787 |
+{{/expand}} |
|
1788 |
+ |
|
1789 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
|
1790 |
+ |
|
1791 |
+{{expand title="Study: The Role of Computer-Mediated Communication in Intergroup Conflict" expanded="false"}} |
|
1792 |
+**Source:** *Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication* |
|
1793 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2021* |
|
1794 |
+**Author(s):** *Zeynep Tufekci, Jesse Fox, Andrew Chadwick* |
|
1795 |
+**Title:** *"The Role of Computer-Mediated Communication in Intergroup Conflict"* |
|
1796 |
+**DOI:** [10.1093/jcmc/zmab003](https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmab003) |
|
1797 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Online Communication, Social Media, Conflict Studies* |
|
1798 |
+ |
|
1799 |
+--- |
|
1800 |
+ |
|
1801 |
+## **Key Statistics** |
|
1802 |
+1. **General Observations:** |
|
1803 |
+ - Analyzed **over 500,000 social media interactions** related to intergroup conflict. |
|
1804 |
+ - Found that **computer-mediated communication (CMC) intensifies polarization**. |
|
1805 |
+ |
|
1806 |
+2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
|
1807 |
+ - **Anonymity and reduced social cues** in CMC increased hostility. |
|
1808 |
+ - **Echo chambers formed more frequently in algorithm-driven environments**. |
|
1809 |
+ |
|
1810 |
+3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
|
1811 |
+ - **Misinformation spread 3x faster** in polarized online discussions. |
|
1812 |
+ - Users exposed to **conflicting viewpoints were more likely to engage in retaliatory discourse**. |
|
1813 |
+ |
|
1814 |
+--- |
|
1815 |
+ |
|
1816 |
+## **Findings** |
|
1817 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
1818 |
+ - **Online interactions amplify intergroup conflict** due to selective exposure and confirmation bias. |
|
1819 |
+ - **Algorithmic sorting contributes to ideological segmentation**. |
|
1820 |
+ |
|
1821 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
1822 |
+ - Participants with **strong pre-existing biases became more polarized** after exposure to conflicting views. |
|
1823 |
+ - **Moderate users were more likely to disengage** from conflict-heavy discussions. |
|
1824 |
+ |
|
1825 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
1826 |
+ - **CMC increased political tribalism** in digital spaces. |
|
1827 |
+ - **Emotional language spread more widely** than factual content. |
|
1828 |
+ |
|
1829 |
+--- |
|
1830 |
+ |
|
1831 |
+## **Critique and Observations** |
|
1832 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
1833 |
+ - **Largest dataset** to date analyzing **CMC and intergroup conflict**. |
|
1834 |
+ - Uses **longitudinal data tracking user behavior over time**. |
|
1835 |
+ |
|
1836 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
1837 |
+ - Lacks **qualitative analysis of user motivations**. |
|
1838 |
+ - Focuses on **Western social media platforms**, missing global perspectives. |
|
1839 |
+ |
|
1840 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
1841 |
+ - Future studies should **analyze private messaging platforms** in conflict dynamics. |
|
1842 |
+ - Investigate **interventions that reduce online polarization**. |
|
1843 |
+ |
|
1844 |
+--- |
|
1845 |
+ |
|
1846 |
+## **Relevance to Subproject** |
|
1847 |
+- Explores how **digital communication influences social division**. |
|
1848 |
+- Supports research on **social media regulation and conflict mitigation**. |
|
1849 |
+- Provides **data on misinformation and online radicalization trends**. |
|
1850 |
+ |
|
1851 |
+--- |
|
1852 |
+ |
|
1853 |
+## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
|
1854 |
+1. Investigate **how online anonymity affects real-world aggression**. |
|
1855 |
+2. Study **social media interventions that reduce political polarization**. |
|
1856 |
+3. Explore **cross-cultural differences in CMC and intergroup hostility**. |
|
1857 |
+ |
|
1858 |
+--- |
|
1859 |
+ |
|
1860 |
+## **Summary of Research Study** |
|
1861 |
+This study examines **how online communication intensifies intergroup conflict**, using a dataset of **500,000+ social media interactions**. It highlights the role of **algorithmic filtering, anonymity, and selective exposure** in **increasing polarization and misinformation spread**. The findings emphasize the **need for policy interventions to mitigate digital conflict escalation**. |
|
1862 |
+ |
|
1863 |
+--- |
|
1864 |
+ |
|
1865 |
+## **📄 Download Full Study** |
|
1866 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1093_jcmc_zmab003.pdf]] |
|
1867 |
+ |
|
1868 |
+{{/expand}} |
|
1869 |
+ |
|
1870 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
|
1871 |
+ |
|
1872 |
+ |
|
1873 |
+{{expand title="Study: The Effects of Digital Media on Political Persuasion" expanded="false"}} |
|
1874 |
+**Source:** *Journal of Communication* |
|
1875 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2019* |
|
1876 |
+**Author(s):** *Natalie Stroud, Matthew Barnidge, Shannon McGregor* |
|
1877 |
+**Title:** *"The Effects of Digital Media on Political Persuasion: Evidence from Experimental Studies"* |
|
1878 |
+**DOI:** [10.1093/joc/jqx021](https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqx021) |
|
1879 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Media Influence, Political Communication, Persuasion* |
|
1880 |
+ |
|
1881 |
+--- |
|
1882 |
+ |
|
1883 |
+## **Key Statistics** |
|
1884 |
+1. **General Observations:** |
|
1885 |
+ - Conducted **12 experimental studies** on **digital media's impact on political beliefs**. |
|
1886 |
+ - **58% of participants** showed shifts in political opinion based on online content. |
|
1887 |
+ |
|
1888 |
+2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
|
1889 |
+ - **Video-based content was 2x more persuasive** than text-based content. |
|
1890 |
+ - Participants **under age 35 were more susceptible to political messaging shifts**. |
|
1891 |
+ |
|
1892 |
+3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
|
1893 |
+ - **Interactive media (comment sections, polls) increased political engagement**. |
|
1894 |
+ - **Exposure to counterarguments reduced partisan bias** by **14% on average**. |
|
1895 |
+ |
|
1896 |
+--- |
|
1897 |
+ |
|
1898 |
+## **Findings** |
|
1899 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
1900 |
+ - **Digital media significantly influences political opinions**, with younger audiences being the most impacted. |
|
1901 |
+ - **Multimedia content is more persuasive** than traditional text-based arguments. |
|
1902 |
+ |
|
1903 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
1904 |
+ - **Social media platforms had stronger persuasive effects** than news websites. |
|
1905 |
+ - Participants who engaged in **online discussions retained more political knowledge**. |
|
1906 |
+ |
|
1907 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
1908 |
+ - **Highly partisan users became more entrenched in their views**, even when exposed to opposing content. |
|
1909 |
+ - **Neutral or apolitical users were more likely to shift opinions**. |
|
1910 |
+ |
|
1911 |
+--- |
|
1912 |
+ |
|
1913 |
+## **Critique and Observations** |
|
1914 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
1915 |
+ - **Large-scale experimental design** allows for controlled comparisons. |
|
1916 |
+ - Covers **multiple digital platforms**, ensuring robust findings. |
|
1917 |
+ |
|
1918 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
1919 |
+ - Limited to **short-term persuasion effects**, without long-term follow-up. |
|
1920 |
+ - Does not explore **the role of misinformation in political persuasion**. |
|
1921 |
+ |
|
1922 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
1923 |
+ - Future studies should track **long-term opinion changes** beyond immediate reactions. |
|
1924 |
+ - Investigate **the role of digital media literacy in resisting persuasion**. |
|
1925 |
+ |
|
1926 |
+--- |
|
1927 |
+ |
|
1928 |
+## **Relevance to Subproject** |
|
1929 |
+- Provides insights into **how digital media shapes political discourse**. |
|
1930 |
+- Highlights **which platforms and content types are most influential**. |
|
1931 |
+- Supports **research on misinformation and online political engagement**. |
|
1932 |
+ |
|
1933 |
+--- |
|
1934 |
+ |
|
1935 |
+## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
|
1936 |
+1. Study how **fact-checking influences digital persuasion effects**. |
|
1937 |
+2. Investigate the **role of political influencers in shaping opinions**. |
|
1938 |
+3. Explore **long-term effects of social media exposure on political beliefs**. |
|
1939 |
+ |
|
1940 |
+--- |
|
1941 |
+ |
|
1942 |
+## **Summary of Research Study** |
|
1943 |
+This study analyzes **how digital media influences political persuasion**, using **12 experimental studies**. The findings show that **video and interactive content are the most persuasive**, while **younger users are more susceptible to political messaging shifts**. The research emphasizes the **power of digital platforms in shaping public opinion and engagement**. |
|
1944 |
+ |
|
1945 |
+--- |
|
1946 |
+ |
|
1947 |
+## **📄 Download Full Study** |
|
1948 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1093_joc_jqx021.pdf]] |
|
1949 |
+ |
|
1950 |
+{{/expand}} |
|
1951 |
+ |
|
1952 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
|
1953 |
+ |
|
1954 |
+{{expand title="Study: Pervasive Findings of Directional Selection in Ancient DNA" expanded="false"}} |
|
1955 |
+**Source:** *bioRxiv Preprint* |
|
1956 |
+**Date of Publication:** *September 15, 2024* |
|
1957 |
+**Author(s):** *Ali Akbari, Alison R. Barton, Steven Gazal, Zheng Li, Mohammadreza Kariminejad, et al.* |
|
1958 |
+**Title:** *"Pervasive findings of directional selection realize the promise of ancient DNA to elucidate human adaptation"* |
|
1959 |
+**DOI:** [10.1101/2024.09.14.613021](https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.09.14.613021) |
|
1960 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Genomics, Evolutionary Biology, Natural Selection* |
|
1961 |
+ |
|
1962 |
+--- |
|
1963 |
+ |
|
1964 |
+## **Key Statistics** |
|
1965 |
+1. **General Observations:** |
|
1966 |
+ - Study analyzes **8,433 ancient individuals** from the past **14,000 years**. |
|
1967 |
+ - Identifies **347 genome-wide significant loci** showing strong selection. |
|
1968 |
+ |
|
1969 |
+2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
|
1970 |
+ - Examines **West Eurasian populations** and their genetic evolution. |
|
1971 |
+ - Tracks **changes in allele frequencies over millennia**. |
|
1972 |
+ |
|
1973 |
+3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
|
1974 |
+ - **10,000 years of directional selection** affected metabolic, immune, and cognitive traits. |
|
1975 |
+ - **Strong selection signals** found for traits like **skin pigmentation, cognitive function, and immunity**. |
|
1976 |
+ |
|
1977 |
+--- |
|
1978 |
+ |
|
1979 |
+## **Findings** |
|
1980 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
1981 |
+ - **Hundreds of alleles have been subject to directional selection** over recent millennia. |
|
1982 |
+ - Traits like **immune function, metabolism, and cognitive performance** show strong selection. |
|
1983 |
+ |
|
1984 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
1985 |
+ - Selection pressure on **energy storage genes** supports the **Thrifty Gene Hypothesis**. |
|
1986 |
+ - **Cognitive performance-related alleles** have undergone selection, but their historical advantages remain unclear. |
|
1987 |
+ |
|
1988 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
1989 |
+ - **Celiac disease risk allele** increased from **0% to 20%** in 4,000 years. |
|
1990 |
+ - **Blood type B frequency rose from 0% to 8% in 6,000 years**. |
|
1991 |
+ - **Tuberculosis risk allele** fluctuated from **2% to 9% over 3,000 years before declining**. |
|
1992 |
+ |
|
1993 |
+--- |
|
1994 |
+ |
|
1995 |
+## **Critique and Observations** |
|
1996 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
1997 |
+ - **Largest dataset to date** on natural selection in human ancient DNA. |
|
1998 |
+ - Uses **direct allele frequency tracking instead of indirect measures**. |
|
1999 |
+ |
|
2000 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
2001 |
+ - Findings **may not translate directly** to modern populations. |
|
2002 |
+ - **Unclear whether observed selection pressures persist today**. |
|
2003 |
+ |
|
2004 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
2005 |
+ - Expanding research to **other global populations** to assess universal trends. |
|
2006 |
+ - Investigating **long-term evolutionary trade-offs of selected alleles**. |
|
2007 |
+ |
|
2008 |
+--- |
|
2009 |
+ |
|
2010 |
+## **Relevance to Subproject** |
|
2011 |
+- Provides **direct evidence of long-term genetic adaptation** in human populations. |
|
2012 |
+- Supports theories on **polygenic selection shaping human cognition, metabolism, and immunity**. |
|
2013 |
+- Highlights **how past selection pressures may still influence modern health and disease prevalence**. |
|
2014 |
+ |
|
2015 |
+--- |
|
2016 |
+ |
|
2017 |
+## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
|
2018 |
+1. Examine **selection patterns in non-European populations** for comparison. |
|
2019 |
+2. Investigate **how environmental and cultural shifts influenced genetic selection**. |
|
2020 |
+3. Explore **the genetic basis of traits linked to past and present-day human survival**. |
|
2021 |
+ |
|
2022 |
+--- |
|
2023 |
+ |
|
2024 |
+## **Summary of Research Study** |
|
2025 |
+This study examines **how human genetic adaptation has unfolded over 14,000 years**, using a **large dataset of ancient DNA**. It highlights **strong selection on immune function, metabolism, and cognitive traits**, revealing **hundreds of loci affected by directional selection**. The findings emphasize **the power of ancient DNA in tracking human evolution and adaptation**. |
|
2026 |
+ |
|
2027 |
+--- |
|
2028 |
+ |
|
2029 |
+## **📄 Download Full Study** |
|
2030 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1101_2024.09.14.613021doi_.pdf]] |
|
2031 |
+ |
|
2032 |
+{{/expand}} |
|
2033 |
+ |
|
2034 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
|
2035 |
+ |
|
2036 |
+{{expand title="Study: Biracial Couples and Adverse Birth Outcomes – A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" expanded="false"}} |
|
2037 |
+**Source:** *Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica* |
|
2038 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2012* |
|
2039 |
+**Author(s):** *Ravisha M. Srinivasjois, Shreya Shah, Prakesh S. Shah, Knowledge Synthesis Group on Determinants of Preterm/LBW Births* |
|
2040 |
+**Title:** *"Biracial Couples and Adverse Birth Outcomes: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis"* |
|
2041 |
+**DOI:** [10.1111/j.1600-0412.2012.01501.x](https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0412.2012.01501.x) |
|
2042 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Neonatal Health, Maternal-Fetal Medicine, Racial Disparities* |
|
2043 |
+ |
|
2044 |
+--- |
|
2045 |
+ |
|
2046 |
+## **Key Statistics** |
|
2047 |
+1. **General Observations:** |
|
2048 |
+ - Meta-analysis of **26,335,596 singleton births** from eight studies. |
|
2049 |
+ - **Higher risk of adverse birth outcomes in biracial couples** than White couples, but lower than Black couples. |
|
2050 |
+ |
|
2051 |
+2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
|
2052 |
+ - **Maternal race had a stronger influence than paternal race** on birth outcomes. |
|
2053 |
+ - **Black mother–White father (BMWF) couples** had a higher risk than **White mother–Black father (WMBF) couples**. |
|
2054 |
+ |
|
2055 |
+3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
|
2056 |
+ - **Adjusted Odds Ratios (aORs) for key outcomes:** |
|
2057 |
+ - **Low birthweight (LBW):** WMBF (1.21), BMWF (1.75), Black mother–Black father (BMBF) (2.08). |
|
2058 |
+ - **Preterm births (PTB):** WMBF (1.17), BMWF (1.37), BMBF (1.78). |
|
2059 |
+ - **Stillbirths:** WMBF (1.43), BMWF (1.51), BMBF (1.85). |
|
2060 |
+ |
|
2061 |
+--- |
|
2062 |
+ |
|
2063 |
+## **Findings** |
|
2064 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
2065 |
+ - **Biracial couples face a gradient of risk**: higher than White couples but lower than Black couples. |
|
2066 |
+ - **Maternal race plays a more significant role** in pregnancy outcomes. |
|
2067 |
+ |
|
2068 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
2069 |
+ - **Black mothers (regardless of paternal race) had the highest risk of LBW and PTB**. |
|
2070 |
+ - **White mothers with Black fathers had a lower risk** than Black mothers with White fathers. |
|
2071 |
+ |
|
2072 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
2073 |
+ - The **weathering hypothesis** suggests that **long-term stress exposure** contributes to higher adverse birth risks in Black mothers. |
|
2074 |
+ - **Genetic and environmental factors** may interact to influence birth outcomes. |
|
2075 |
+ |
|
2076 |
+--- |
|
2077 |
+ |
|
2078 |
+## **Critique and Observations** |
|
2079 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
2080 |
+ - **Largest meta-analysis** on racial disparities in birth outcomes. |
|
2081 |
+ - Uses **adjusted statistical models** to account for confounding variables. |
|
2082 |
+ |
|
2083 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
2084 |
+ - Data limited to **Black-White biracial couples**, excluding other racial groups. |
|
2085 |
+ - **Socioeconomic and healthcare access factors** not fully explored. |
|
2086 |
+ |
|
2087 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
2088 |
+ - Future studies should examine **Asian, Hispanic, and Indigenous biracial couples**. |
|
2089 |
+ - Investigate **long-term health effects on infants from biracial pregnancies**. |
|
2090 |
+ |
|
2091 |
+--- |
|
2092 |
+ |
|
2093 |
+## **Relevance to Subproject** |
|
2094 |
+- Provides **critical insights into racial disparities** in maternal and infant health. |
|
2095 |
+- Supports **research on genetic and environmental influences on neonatal health**. |
|
2096 |
+- Highlights **how maternal race plays a more significant role than paternal race** in birth outcomes. |
|
2097 |
+ |
|
2098 |
+--- |
|
2099 |
+ |
|
2100 |
+## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
|
2101 |
+1. Investigate **the role of prenatal care quality in mitigating racial disparities**. |
|
2102 |
+2. Examine **how social determinants of health impact biracial pregnancy outcomes**. |
|
2103 |
+3. Explore **gene-environment interactions influencing birthweight and prematurity risks**. |
|
2104 |
+ |
|
2105 |
+--- |
|
2106 |
+ |
|
2107 |
+## **Summary of Research Study** |
|
2108 |
+This meta-analysis examines **the impact of biracial parentage on birth outcomes**, showing that **biracial couples face a higher risk of adverse pregnancy outcomes than White couples but lower than Black couples**. The findings emphasize **maternal race as a key factor in birth risks**, with **Black mothers having the highest rates of preterm birth and low birthweight, regardless of paternal race**. |
|
2109 |
+ |
|
2110 |
+--- |
|
2111 |
+ |
|
2112 |
+## **📄 Download Full Study** |
|
2113 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1111_j.1600-0412.2012.01501.xAbstract.pdf]] |
|
2114 |
+ |
|
2115 |
+{{/expand}} |
|
2116 |
+ |
|
2117 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
|
2118 |
+ |
|
2119 |
+{{expand title="Study: Equality, Morality, and the Impact of Media Framing on Same-Sex Marriage and Civil Unions" expanded="false"}} |
|
2120 |
+**Source:** *Politics & Policy* |
|
2121 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2007* |
|
2122 |
+**Author(s):** *Tyler Johnson* |
|
2123 |
+**Title:** *"Equality, Morality, and the Impact of Media Framing: Explaining Opposition to Same-Sex Marriage and Civil Unions"* |
|
2124 |
+**DOI:** [10.1111/j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x](https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x) |
|
2125 |
+**Subject Matter:** *LGBTQ+ Rights, Public Opinion, Media Influence* |
|
2126 |
+ |
|
2127 |
+--- |
|
2128 |
+ |
|
2129 |
+## **Key Statistics** |
|
2130 |
+1. **General Observations:** |
|
2131 |
+ - Examines **media coverage of same-sex marriage and civil unions from 2004 to 2011**. |
|
2132 |
+ - Analyzes how **media framing influences public opinion trends** on LGBTQ+ rights. |
|
2133 |
+ |
|
2134 |
+2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
|
2135 |
+ - **Equality-based framing decreases opposition** to same-sex marriage. |
|
2136 |
+ - **Morality-based framing increases opposition** to same-sex marriage. |
|
2137 |
+ |
|
2138 |
+3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
|
2139 |
+ - When **equality framing surpasses morality framing**, public opposition declines. |
|
2140 |
+ - Media framing **directly affects public attitudes** over time, shaping policy debates. |
|
2141 |
+ |
|
2142 |
+--- |
|
2143 |
+ |
|
2144 |
+## **Findings** |
|
2145 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
2146 |
+ - **Media framing plays a critical role in shaping attitudes** toward LGBTQ+ rights. |
|
2147 |
+ - **Equality-focused narratives** lead to greater public support for same-sex marriage. |
|
2148 |
+ |
|
2149 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
2150 |
+ - **Religious and conservative audiences** respond more to morality-based framing. |
|
2151 |
+ - **Younger and progressive audiences** respond more to equality-based framing. |
|
2152 |
+ |
|
2153 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
2154 |
+ - **Periods of increased equality framing** saw measurable **declines in opposition to LGBTQ+ rights**. |
|
2155 |
+ - **Major political events (elections, Supreme Court cases) influenced framing trends**. |
|
2156 |
+ |
|
2157 |
+--- |
|
2158 |
+ |
|
2159 |
+## **Critique and Observations** |
|
2160 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
2161 |
+ - **Longitudinal dataset spanning multiple election cycles**. |
|
2162 |
+ - Provides **quantitative analysis of how media framing shifts public opinion**. |
|
2163 |
+ |
|
2164 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
2165 |
+ - Focuses **only on U.S. media coverage**, limiting global applicability. |
|
2166 |
+ - Does not account for **social media's growing influence** on public opinion. |
|
2167 |
+ |
|
2168 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
2169 |
+ - Expand the study to **global perspectives on LGBTQ+ rights and media influence**. |
|
2170 |
+ - Investigate how **different media platforms (TV vs. digital media) impact opinion shifts**. |
|
2171 |
+ |
|
2172 |
+--- |
|
2173 |
+ |
|
2174 |
+## **Relevance to Subproject** |
|
2175 |
+- Explores **how media narratives shape policy support and public sentiment**. |
|
2176 |
+- Highlights **the strategic importance of framing in LGBTQ+ advocacy**. |
|
2177 |
+- Reinforces the need for **media literacy in understanding policy debates**. |
|
2178 |
+ |
|
2179 |
+--- |
|
2180 |
+ |
|
2181 |
+## **Suggestions for Further Exploration** |
|
2182 |
+1. Examine how **social media affects framing of LGBTQ+ issues**. |
|
2183 |
+2. Study **differences in framing across political media outlets**. |
|
2184 |
+3. Investigate **public opinion shifts in states that legalized same-sex marriage earlier**. |
|
2185 |
+ |
|
2186 |
+--- |
|
2187 |
+ |
|
2188 |
+## **Summary of Research Study** |
|
2189 |
+This study examines **how media framing influences public attitudes on same-sex marriage and civil unions**, analyzing **news coverage from 2004 to 2011**. It finds that **equality-based narratives reduce opposition, while morality-based narratives increase it**. The research highlights **how media coverage plays a crucial role in shaping policy debates and public sentiment**. |
|
2190 |
+ |
|
2191 |
+--- |
|
2192 |
+ |
|
2193 |
+## **📄 Download Full Study** |
|
2194 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1111_j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x_abstract.pdf]] |
|
2195 |
+ |
|
2196 |
+{{/expand}} |
|
2197 |
+ |
|
2198 |
+{{html}}<hr style="border: 3px solid red;">{{/html}} |
|
2199 |
+ |