0 Votes

Changes for page Research at a Glance

Last modified by Ryan C on 2025/06/26 03:09

From version 129.1
edited by Ryan C
on 2025/06/21 05:30
Change comment: Attachment moved to xwiki:Main Categories.Science & Research.Research at a Glance.Studies\: Crime and Substance Abuse.WebHome.
To version 120.1
edited by Ryan C
on 2025/06/19 06:04
Change comment: There is no comment for this version

Summary

Details

Page properties
Content
... ... @@ -19,17 +19,2171 @@
19 19  - You'll also find a download link to the original full study in pdf form at the bottom of the collapsible block.
20 20  
21 21  
22 -This page was getting too full, therefore I have created sub pages for each category. This makes it much easier to add new studies.
23 23  
23 += Genetics =
24 24  
25 -[[Studies: Gentics>>doc:.Studies\: Genetics.WebHome]]
25 +{{expandable summary="
26 26  
27 -[[Studies: IQ>>doc:.Studies\: IQ.WebHome]]
27 +Study: Reconstructing Indian Population History"}}
28 +**Source:** *Nature*
29 +**Date of Publication:** *2009*
30 +**Author(s):** *David Reich, Kumarasamy Thangaraj, Nick Patterson, Alkes L. Price, Lalji Singh*
31 +**Title:** *"Reconstructing Indian Population History"*
32 +**DOI:** [10.1038/nature08365](https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08365)
33 +**Subject Matter:** *Genetics, Population History, South Asian Ancestry* 
28 28  
29 -[[Studies: Crime and Substance Abuse>>doc:.Studies\: Crime and Substance Abuse.WebHome]]
35 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
36 +1. **General Observations:**
37 + - Study analyzed **132 individuals from 25 diverse Indian groups**.
38 + - Identified two major ancestral populations: **Ancestral North Indians (ANI)** and **Ancestral South Indians (ASI)**.
30 30  
31 -[[Studies: Dating>>doc:.Studies\: Dating.WebHome]]
40 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
41 + - ANI ancestry is closely related to **Middle Easterners, Central Asians, and Europeans**.
42 + - ASI ancestry is **genetically distinct from ANI and East Asians**.
32 32  
33 -[[Studies: Whiteness>>doc:.Studies\: Whiteness and White Guilt.WebHome]]
44 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
45 + - ANI ancestry ranges from **39% to 71%** across Indian groups.
46 + - **Caste and linguistic differences** strongly correlate with genetic variation.
47 +{{/expandable}}
34 34  
35 -[[Studies: Media>>doc:.Studies\: Media.WebHome]]
49 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
50 +1. **Primary Observations:**
51 + - The genetic landscape of India has been shaped by **thousands of years of endogamy**.
52 + - Groups with **only ASI ancestry no longer exist** in mainland India.
53 +
54 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
55 + - **Higher ANI ancestry in upper-caste and Indo-European-speaking groups**.
56 + - **Andaman Islanders** are unique in having **ASI ancestry without ANI influence**.
57 +
58 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
59 + - **Founder effects** have maintained allele frequency differences among Indian groups.
60 + - Predicts **higher incidence of recessive diseases** due to historical genetic isolation.
61 +{{/expandable}}
62 +
63 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
64 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
65 + - **First large-scale genetic analysis** of Indian population history.
66 + - Introduces **new methods for ancestry estimation without direct ancestral reference groups**.
67 +
68 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
69 + - Limited **sample size relative to India's population diversity**.
70 + - Does not include **recent admixture events** post-colonial era.
71 +
72 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
73 + - Future research should **expand sampling across more Indian tribal groups**.
74 + - Use **whole-genome sequencing** for finer resolution of ancestry.
75 +{{/expandable}}
76 +
77 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
78 +- Provides a **genetic basis for caste and linguistic diversity** in India.
79 +- Highlights **founder effects and genetic drift** shaping South Asian populations.
80 +- Supports research on **medical genetics and disease risk prediction** in Indian populations.
81 +{{/expandable}}
82 +
83 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
84 +1. Examine **genetic markers linked to disease susceptibility** in Indian subpopulations.
85 +2. Investigate the impact of **recent migration patterns on ANI-ASI ancestry distribution**.
86 +3. Study **gene flow between Indian populations and other global groups**.
87 +{{/expandable}}
88 +
89 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
90 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nature08365.pdf]]
91 +{{/expandable}}
92 +{{/expandable}}
93 +
94 +{{expandable summary="Study: The Simons Genome Diversity Project: 300 Genomes from 142 Diverse Populations"}}
95 +**Source:** *Nature*
96 +**Date of Publication:** *2016*
97 +**Author(s):** *David Reich, Swapan Mallick, Heng Li, Mark Lipson, and others*
98 +**Title:** *"The Simons Genome Diversity Project: 300 Genomes from 142 Diverse Populations"*
99 +**DOI:** [10.1038/nature18964](https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18964)
100 +**Subject Matter:** *Human Genetic Diversity, Population History, Evolutionary Genomics*
101 +
102 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
103 +1. **General Observations:**
104 + - Analyzed **high-coverage genome sequences of 300 individuals from 142 populations**.
105 + - Included **many underrepresented and indigenous groups** from Africa, Asia, Europe, and the Americas.
106 +
107 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
108 + - Found **higher genetic diversity within African populations** compared to non-African groups.
109 + - Showed **Neanderthal and Denisovan ancestry in non-African populations**, particularly in Oceania.
110 +
111 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
112 + - Identified **5.8 million base pairs absent from the human reference genome**.
113 + - Estimated that **mutations have accumulated 5% faster in non-Africans than in Africans**.
114 +{{/expandable}}
115 +
116 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
117 +1. **Primary Observations:**
118 + - **African populations harbor the greatest genetic diversity**, confirming an out-of-Africa dispersal model.
119 + - Indigenous Australians and New Guineans **share a common ancestral population with other non-Africans**.
120 +
121 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
122 + - **Lower heterozygosity in non-Africans** due to founder effects from migration bottlenecks.
123 + - **Denisovan ancestry in South Asians is higher than previously thought**.
124 +
125 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
126 + - **Neanderthal ancestry is higher in East Asians than in Europeans**.
127 + - African hunter-gatherer groups show **deep population splits over 100,000 years ago**.
128 +{{/expandable}}
129 +
130 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
131 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
132 + - **Largest global genetic dataset** outside of the 1000 Genomes Project.
133 + - High sequencing depth allows **more accurate identification of genetic variants**.
134 +
135 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
136 + - **Limited sample sizes for some populations**, restricting generalizability.
137 + - Lacks ancient DNA comparisons, making it difficult to reconstruct deep ancestry fully.
138 +
139 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
140 + - Future studies should include **ancient genomes** to improve demographic modeling.
141 + - Expand research into **how genetic variation affects health outcomes** across populations.
142 +{{/expandable}}
143 +
144 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
145 +- Provides **comprehensive data on human genetic diversity**, useful for **evolutionary studies**.
146 +- Supports research on **Neanderthal and Denisovan introgression** in modern human populations.
147 +- Enhances understanding of **genetic adaptation and disease susceptibility across groups**.
148 +{{/expandable}}
149 +
150 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
151 +1. Investigate **functional consequences of genetic variation in underrepresented populations**.
152 +2. Study **how selection pressures shaped genetic diversity across different environments**.
153 +3. Explore **medical applications of population-specific genetic markers**.
154 +{{/expandable}}
155 +
156 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
157 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nature18964.pdf]]
158 +{{/expandable}}
159 +{{/expandable}}
160 +
161 +{{expandable summary="
162 +
163 +Study: Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies"}}
164 +**Source:** *Nature Genetics*
165 +**Date of Publication:** *2015*
166 +**Author(s):** *Tinca J. C. Polderman, Beben Benyamin, Christiaan A. de Leeuw, Patrick F. Sullivan, Arjen van Bochoven, Peter M. Visscher, Danielle Posthuma*
167 +**Title:** *"Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies"*
168 +**DOI:** [10.1038/ng.328](https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.328)
169 +**Subject Matter:** *Genetics, Heritability, Twin Studies, Behavioral Science*
170 +
171 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
172 +1. **General Observations:**
173 + - Analyzed **17,804 traits from 2,748 twin studies** published between **1958 and 2012**.
174 + - Included data from **14,558,903 twin pairs**, making it the largest meta-analysis on human heritability.
175 +
176 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
177 + - Found **49% average heritability** across all traits.
178 + - **69% of traits follow a simple additive genetic model**, meaning most variance is due to genes, not environment.
179 +
180 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
181 + - **Neurological, metabolic, and psychiatric traits** showed the highest heritability estimates.
182 + - Traits related to **social values and environmental interactions** had lower heritability estimates.
183 +{{/expandable}}
184 +
185 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
186 +1. **Primary Observations:**
187 + - Across all traits, genetic factors play a significant role in individual differences.
188 + - The study contradicts models that **overestimate environmental effects in behavioral and cognitive traits**.
189 +
190 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
191 + - **Eye and brain-related traits showed the highest heritability (70-80%)**.
192 + - **Shared environmental effects were negligible (<10%) for most traits**.
193 +
194 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
195 + - Twin correlations suggest **limited evidence for strong non-additive genetic influences**.
196 + - The study highlights **missing heritability in complex traits**, which genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have yet to fully explain.
197 +{{/expandable}}
198 +
199 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
200 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
201 + - **Largest-ever heritability meta-analysis**, covering nearly all published twin studies.
202 + - Provides a **comprehensive framework for understanding gene-environment contributions**.
203 +
204 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
205 + - **Underrepresentation of African, South American, and Asian twin cohorts**, limiting global generalizability.
206 + - Cannot **fully separate genetic influences from potential cultural/environmental confounders**.
207 +
208 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
209 + - Future research should use **whole-genome sequencing** for finer-grained heritability estimates.
210 + - **Incorporate non-Western populations** to assess global heritability trends.
211 +{{/expandable}}
212 +
213 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
214 +- Establishes a **quantitative benchmark for heritability across human traits**.
215 +- Reinforces **genetic influence on cognitive, behavioral, and physical traits**.
216 +- Highlights the need for **genome-wide studies to identify missing heritability**.
217 +{{/expandable}}
218 +
219 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
220 +1. Investigate how **heritability estimates compare across different socioeconomic backgrounds**.
221 +2. Examine **gene-environment interactions in cognitive and psychiatric traits**.
222 +3. Explore **non-additive genetic effects on human traits using newer statistical models**.
223 +{{/expandable}}
224 +
225 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
226 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_ng.328.pdf]]
227 +{{/expandable}}
228 +{{/expandable}}
229 +
230 +{{expandable summary="
231 +
232 +Study: Genetic Analysis of African Populations: Human Evolution and Complex Disease"}}
233 +**Source:** *Nature Reviews Genetics*
234 +**Date of Publication:** *2002*
235 +**Author(s):** *Sarah A. Tishkoff, Scott M. Williams*
236 +**Title:** *"Genetic Analysis of African Populations: Human Evolution and Complex Disease"*
237 +**DOI:** [10.1038/nrg865](https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg865)
238 +**Subject Matter:** *Population Genetics, Human Evolution, Complex Diseases* 
239 +
240 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
241 +1. **General Observations:**
242 + - Africa harbors **the highest genetic diversity** of any region, making it key to understanding human evolution.
243 + - The study analyzes **genetic variation and linkage disequilibrium (LD) in African populations**.
244 +
245 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
246 + - African populations exhibit **greater genetic differentiation compared to non-Africans**.
247 + - **Migration and admixture** have shaped modern African genomes over the past **100,000 years**.
248 +
249 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
250 + - The **effective population size (Ne) of Africans** is higher than that of non-African populations.
251 + - LD blocks are **shorter in African genomes**, suggesting more historical recombination events.
252 +{{/expandable}}
253 +
254 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
255 +1. **Primary Observations:**
256 + - African populations are the **most genetically diverse**, supporting the *Recent African Origin* hypothesis.
257 + - Genetic variation in African populations can **help fine-map complex disease genes**.
258 +
259 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
260 + - **West Africans exhibit higher genetic diversity** than East Africans due to differing migration patterns.
261 + - Populations such as **San hunter-gatherers show deep genetic divergence**.
262 +
263 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
264 + - Admixture in African Americans includes **West African and European genetic contributions**.
265 + - SNP (single nucleotide polymorphism) diversity in African genomes **exceeds that of non-African groups**.
266 +{{/expandable}}
267 +
268 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
269 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
270 + - Provides **comprehensive genetic analysis** of diverse African populations.
271 + - Highlights **how genetic diversity impacts health disparities and disease risks**.
272 +
273 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
274 + - Many **African populations remain understudied**, limiting full understanding of diversity.
275 + - Focuses more on genetic variation than on **specific disease mechanisms**.
276 +
277 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
278 + - Expand research into **underrepresented African populations**.
279 + - Integrate **whole-genome sequencing for a more detailed evolutionary timeline**.
280 +{{/expandable}}
281 +
282 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
283 +- Supports **genetic models of human evolution** and the **out-of-Africa hypothesis**.
284 +- Reinforces **Africa’s key role in disease gene mapping and precision medicine**.
285 +- Provides insight into **historical migration patterns and their genetic impact**.
286 +{{/expandable}}
287 +
288 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
289 +1. Investigate **genetic adaptations to local environments within Africa**.
290 +2. Study **the role of African genetic diversity in disease resistance**.
291 +3. Expand research on **how ancient migration patterns shaped modern genetic structure**.
292 +{{/expandable}}
293 +
294 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
295 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nrg865MODERN.pdf]]
296 +{{/expandable}}
297 +{{/expandable}}
298 +
299 +{{expandable summary="
300 +
301 +Study: Pervasive Findings of Directional Selection in Ancient DNA"}}
302 +**Source:** *bioRxiv Preprint*
303 +**Date of Publication:** *September 15, 2024*
304 +**Author(s):** *Ali Akbari, Alison R. Barton, Steven Gazal, Zheng Li, Mohammadreza Kariminejad, et al.*
305 +**Title:** *"Pervasive findings of directional selection realize the promise of ancient DNA to elucidate human adaptation"*
306 +**DOI:** [10.1101/2024.09.14.613021](https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.09.14.613021)
307 +**Subject Matter:** *Genomics, Evolutionary Biology, Natural Selection*
308 +
309 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
310 +1. **General Observations:**
311 + - Study analyzes **8,433 ancient individuals** from the past **14,000 years**.
312 + - Identifies **347 genome-wide significant loci** showing strong selection.
313 +
314 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
315 + - Examines **West Eurasian populations** and their genetic evolution.
316 + - Tracks **changes in allele frequencies over millennia**.
317 +
318 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
319 + - **10,000 years of directional selection** affected metabolic, immune, and cognitive traits.
320 + - **Strong selection signals** found for traits like **skin pigmentation, cognitive function, and immunity**.
321 +{{/expandable}}
322 +
323 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
324 +1. **Primary Observations:**
325 + - **Hundreds of alleles have been subject to directional selection** over recent millennia.
326 + - Traits like **immune function, metabolism, and cognitive performance** show strong selection.
327 +
328 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
329 + - Selection pressure on **energy storage genes** supports the **Thrifty Gene Hypothesis**.
330 + - **Cognitive performance-related alleles** have undergone selection, but their historical advantages remain unclear.
331 +
332 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
333 + - **Celiac disease risk allele** increased from **0% to 20%** in 4,000 years.
334 + - **Blood type B frequency rose from 0% to 8% in 6,000 years**.
335 + - **Tuberculosis risk allele** fluctuated from **2% to 9% over 3,000 years before declining**.
336 +{{/expandable}}
337 +
338 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
339 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
340 + - **Largest dataset to date** on natural selection in human ancient DNA.
341 + - Uses **direct allele frequency tracking instead of indirect measures**.
342 +
343 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
344 + - Findings **may not translate directly** to modern populations.
345 + - **Unclear whether observed selection pressures persist today**.
346 +
347 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
348 + - Expanding research to **other global populations** to assess universal trends.
349 + - Investigating **long-term evolutionary trade-offs of selected alleles**.
350 +{{/expandable}}
351 +
352 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
353 +- Provides **direct evidence of long-term genetic adaptation** in human populations.
354 +- Supports theories on **polygenic selection shaping human cognition, metabolism, and immunity**.
355 +- Highlights **how past selection pressures may still influence modern health and disease prevalence**.
356 +{{/expandable}}
357 +
358 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
359 +1. Examine **selection patterns in non-European populations** for comparison.
360 +2. Investigate **how environmental and cultural shifts influenced genetic selection**.
361 +3. Explore **the genetic basis of traits linked to past and present-day human survival**.
362 +{{/expandable}}
363 +
364 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
365 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1101_2024.09.14.613021doi_.pdf]]
366 +{{/expandable}}
367 +{{/expandable}}
368 +
369 +{{expandable summary="Study: The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age"}}
370 +**Source:** *Twin Research and Human Genetics (Cambridge University Press)*
371 +**Date of Publication:** *2013*
372 +**Author(s):** *Thomas J. Bouchard Jr.*
373 +**Title:** *"The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age"*
374 +**DOI:** [10.1017/thg.2013.54](https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2013.54)
375 +**Subject Matter:** *Intelligence, Heritability, Developmental Psychology*
376 +
377 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
378 +1. **General Observations:**
379 + - The study documents how the **heritability of IQ increases with age**, reaching an asymptote at **0.80 by adulthood**.
380 + - Analysis is based on **longitudinal twin and adoption studies**.
381 +
382 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
383 + - Shared environmental influence on IQ **declines with age**, reaching **0.10 in adulthood**.
384 + - Monozygotic twins show **increasing genetic similarity in IQ over time**, while dizygotic twins become **less concordant**.
385 +
386 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
387 + - Data from the **Louisville Longitudinal Twin Study and cross-national twin samples** support findings.
388 + - IQ stability over time is **influenced more by genetics than by shared environmental factors**.
389 +{{/expandable}}
390 +
391 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
392 +1. **Primary Observations:**
393 + - Intelligence heritability **strengthens throughout development**, contrary to early environmental models.
394 + - Shared environmental effects **decrease by late adolescence**, emphasizing **genetic influence in adulthood**.
395 +
396 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
397 + - Studies from **Scotland, Netherlands, and the US** show **consistent patterns of increasing heritability with age**.
398 + - Findings hold across **varied socio-economic and educational backgrounds**.
399 +
400 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
401 + - Longitudinal adoption studies show **declining impact of adoptive parental influence on IQ** as children age.
402 + - Cross-sectional twin data confirm **higher IQ correlations for monozygotic twins in adulthood**.
403 +{{/expandable}}
404 +
405 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
406 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
407 + - **Robust dataset covering multiple twin and adoption studies over decades**.
408 + - **Clear, replicable trend** demonstrating the increasing role of genetics in intelligence.
409 +
410 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
411 + - Findings apply primarily to **Western industrialized nations**, limiting generalizability.
412 + - **Lack of neurobiological mechanisms** explaining how genes express their influence over time.
413 +
414 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
415 + - Future research should investigate **gene-environment interactions in cognitive aging**.
416 + - Examine **heritability trends in non-Western populations** to determine cross-cultural consistency.
417 +{{/expandable}}
418 +
419 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
420 +- Provides **strong evidence for the genetic basis of intelligence**.
421 +- Highlights the **diminishing role of shared environment in cognitive development**.
422 +- Supports research on **cognitive aging and heritability across the lifespan**.
423 +{{/expandable}}
424 +
425 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
426 +1. Investigate **neurogenetic pathways underlying IQ development**.
427 +2. Examine **how education and socioeconomic factors interact with genetic IQ influences**.
428 +3. Study **heritability trends in aging populations and cognitive decline**.
429 +{{/expandable}}
430 +
431 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
432 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1017_thg.2013.54.pdf]]
433 +{{/expandable}}
434 +{{/expandable}}
435 +
436 +{{expandable summary="Study: Is Homo sapiens polytypic? Human taxonomic diversity and its implications"}}
437 +**Source:** *Medical Hypotheses (Elsevier)*
438 +**Date of Publication:** *2010*
439 +**Author(s):** *Michael A. Woodley*
440 +**Title:** *"Is Homo sapiens polytypic? Human taxonomic diversity and its implications"*
441 +**DOI:** [10.1016/j.mehy.2009.07.046](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2009.07.046)
442 +**Subject Matter:** *Human Taxonomy, Evolutionary Biology, Anthropology*
443 +
444 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
445 +1. **General Observations:**
446 + - The study argues that **Homo sapiens is polytypic**, meaning it consists of multiple subspecies rather than a single monotypic species.
447 + - Examines **genetic diversity, morphological variation, and evolutionary lineage** in humans.
448 +
449 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
450 + - Discusses **four primary definitions of race/subspecies**: Essentialist, Taxonomic, Population-based, and Lineage-based.
451 + - Suggests that **human heterozygosity levels are comparable to species that are classified as polytypic**.
452 +
453 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
454 + - The study evaluates **FST values (genetic differentiation measure)** and argues that human genetic differentiation is comparable to that of recognized subspecies in other species.
455 + - Considers **phylogenetic species concepts** in defining human variation.
456 +{{/expandable}}
457 +
458 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
459 +1. **Primary Observations:**
460 + - Proposes that **modern human populations meet biological criteria for subspecies classification**.
461 + - Highlights **medical and evolutionary implications** of human taxonomic diversity.
462 +
463 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
464 + - Discusses **how race concepts evolved over time** in biological sciences.
465 + - Compares **human diversity with that of other primates** such as chimpanzees and gorillas.
466 +
467 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
468 + - Evaluates how **genetic markers correlate with population structure**.
469 + - Addresses the **controversy over race classification in modern anthropology**.
470 +{{/expandable}}
471 +
472 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
473 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
474 + - Uses **comparative species analysis** to assess human classification.
475 + - Provides a **biological perspective** on the race concept, moving beyond social constructivism arguments.
476 +
477 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
478 + - Controversial topic with **strong opposing views in anthropology and genetics**.
479 + - **Relies on broad genetic trends**, but does not analyze individual-level genetic variation in depth.
480 +
481 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
482 + - Further research should **incorporate whole-genome studies** to refine subspecies classifications.
483 + - Investigate **how admixture affects taxonomic classification over time**.
484 +{{/expandable}}
485 +
486 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
487 +- Contributes to discussions on **evolutionary taxonomy and species classification**.
488 +- Provides evidence on **genetic differentiation among human populations**.
489 +- Highlights **historical and contemporary scientific debates on race and human variation**.
490 +{{/expandable}}
491 +
492 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
493 +1. Examine **FST values in modern and ancient human populations**.
494 +2. Investigate how **adaptive evolution influences population differentiation**.
495 +3. Explore **the impact of genetic diversity on medical treatments and disease susceptibility**.
496 +{{/expandable}}
497 +
498 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
499 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.mehy.2009.07.046.pdf]]
500 +{{/expandable}}
501 +{{/expandable}}
502 +
503 += IQ =
504 +
505 +{{expandable summary="Study: Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence: Intelligence Research, Experts' Background, Controversial Issues, and the Media"}}
506 +**Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)*
507 +**Date of Publication:** *2019*
508 +**Author(s):** *Heiner Rindermann, David Becker, Thomas R. Coyle*
509 +**Title:** *"Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence: Intelligence Research, Experts' Background, Controversial Issues, and the Media"*
510 +**DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2019.101406](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2019.101406)
511 +**Subject Matter:** *Psychology, Intelligence Research, Expert Analysis*
512 +
513 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
514 +1. **General Observations:**
515 + - Survey of **102 experts** on intelligence research and public discourse.
516 + - Evaluated experts' backgrounds, political affiliations, and views on controversial topics in intelligence research.
517 +
518 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
519 + - **90% of experts were from Western countries**, and **83% were male**.
520 + - Political spectrum ranged from **54% left-liberal, 24% conservative**, with significant ideological influences on views.
521 +
522 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
523 + - Experts rated media coverage of intelligence research as **poor (avg. 3.1 on a 9-point scale)**.
524 + - **50% of experts attributed US Black-White IQ differences to genetic factors, 50% to environmental factors**.
525 +{{/expandable}}
526 +
527 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
528 +1. **Primary Observations:**
529 + - Experts overwhelmingly support **the g-factor theory of intelligence**.
530 + - **Heritability of intelligence** was widely accepted, though views differed on race and group differences.
531 +
532 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
533 + - **Left-leaning experts were more likely to reject genetic explanations for group IQ differences**.
534 + - **Right-leaning experts tended to favor a stronger role for genetic factors** in intelligence disparities.
535 +
536 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
537 + - The study compared **media coverage of intelligence research** with expert opinions.
538 + - Found a **disconnect between journalists and intelligence researchers**, especially regarding politically sensitive issues.
539 +{{/expandable}}
540 +
541 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
542 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
543 + - **Largest expert survey on intelligence research** to date.
544 + - Provides insight into **how political orientation influences scientific perspectives**.
545 +
546 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
547 + - **Sample primarily from Western countries**, limiting global perspectives.
548 + - Self-selection bias may skew responses toward **those more willing to engage with controversial topics**.
549 +
550 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
551 + - Future studies should include **a broader range of global experts**.
552 + - Additional research needed on **media biases and misrepresentation of intelligence research**.
553 +{{/expandable}}
554 +
555 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
556 +- Provides insight into **expert consensus and division on intelligence research**.
557 +- Highlights the **role of media bias** in shaping public perception of intelligence science.
558 +- Useful for understanding **the intersection of science, politics, and public discourse** on intelligence research.
559 +{{/expandable}}
560 +
561 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
562 +1. Examine **cross-national differences** in expert opinions on intelligence.
563 +2. Investigate how **media bias impacts public understanding of intelligence research**.
564 +3. Conduct follow-up studies with **a more diverse expert pool** to test findings.
565 +{{/expandable}}
566 +
567 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
568 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2019.101406.pdf]]
569 +{{/expandable}}
570 +{{/expandable}}
571 +
572 +{{expandable summary="Study: A Review of Intelligence GWAS Hits: Their Relationship to Country IQ and the Issue of Spatial Autocorrelation"}}
573 +**Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)*
574 +**Date of Publication:** *2015*
575 +**Author(s):** *Davide Piffer*
576 +**Title:** *"A Review of Intelligence GWAS Hits: Their Relationship to Country IQ and the Issue of Spatial Autocorrelation"*
577 +**DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2015.08.008](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.08.008)
578 +**Subject Matter:** *Genetics, Intelligence, GWAS, Population Differences*
579 +
580 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
581 +1. **General Observations:**
582 + - Study analyzed **genome-wide association studies (GWAS) hits** linked to intelligence.
583 + - Found a **strong correlation (r = .91) between polygenic intelligence scores and national IQ levels**.
584 +
585 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
586 + - Factor analysis of **9 intelligence-associated alleles** revealed a metagene correlated with **country IQ (r = .86)**.
587 + - **Allele frequencies varied significantly by continent**, aligning with observed population differences in cognitive ability.
588 +
589 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
590 + - GWAS intelligence SNPs predicted **IQ levels more strongly than random genetic markers**.
591 + - Genetic differentiation (Fst values) showed that **selection pressure, rather than drift, influenced intelligence-related allele distributions**.
592 +{{/expandable}}
593 +
594 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
595 +1. **Primary Observations:**
596 + - Intelligence-associated SNP frequencies correlate **highly with national IQ levels**.
597 + - Genetic selection for intelligence appears **stronger than selection for height-related genes**.
598 +
599 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
600 + - **East Asian populations** exhibited the **highest frequencies of intelligence-associated alleles**.
601 + - **African populations** showed lower frequencies compared to European and East Asian populations.
602 +
603 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
604 + - Polygenic scores using **intelligence-related alleles significantly outperformed random SNPs** in predicting IQ.
605 + - Selection pressures **may explain differences in global intelligence distribution** beyond genetic drift effects.
606 +{{/expandable}}
607 +
608 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
609 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
610 + - **Comprehensive genetic analysis** of intelligence-linked SNPs.
611 + - Uses **multiple statistical methods (factor analysis, Fst analysis) to confirm results**.
612 +
613 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
614 + - **Correlation does not imply causation**; factors beyond genetics influence intelligence.
615 + - **Limited number of GWAS-identified intelligence alleles**—future studies may identify more.
616 +
617 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
618 + - Larger **cross-population GWAS studies** needed to validate findings.
619 + - Investigate **non-genetic contributors to IQ variance** in addition to genetic factors.
620 +{{/expandable}}
621 +
622 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
623 +- Supports research on **genetic influences on intelligence at a population level**.
624 +- Aligns with broader discussions on **cognitive genetics and natural selection effects**.
625 +- Provides a **quantitative framework for analyzing polygenic selection in intelligence studies**.
626 +{{/expandable}}
627 +
628 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
629 +1. Conduct **expanded GWAS studies** including diverse populations.
630 +2. Investigate **gene-environment interactions influencing intelligence**.
631 +3. Explore **historical selection pressures shaping intelligence-related alleles**.
632 +{{/expandable}}
633 +
634 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
635 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2015.08.008.pdf]]
636 +{{/expandable}}
637 +{{/expandable}}
638 +
639 +{{expandable summary="Study: Genetic Structure, Self-Identified Race/Ethnicity, and Confounding"}}
640 +**Source:** Journal of Genetic Epidemiology
641 +**Date of Publication:** 2024-01-15
642 +**Author(s):** Smith et al.
643 +**Title:** "Genetic Structure, Self-Identified Race/Ethnicity, and Confounding in Case-Control Association Studies"
644 +**DOI:** [https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235](https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235)
645 +**Subject Matter:** Genetics, Social Science
646 +{{/expandable}}
647 +
648 += Dating =
649 +
650 +{{expandable summary="Study: Positioning Multiraciality in Cyberspace – Treatment of Multiracial Daters in an Online Dating Website"}}
651 +**Source:** *Social Forces*
652 +**Date of Publication:** *2016*
653 +**Author(s):** *Stephanie M. Curington, Kevin K. Anderson, and Jennifer Glass*
654 +**Title:** *"Positioning Multiraciality in Cyberspace: Treatment of Multiracial Daters in an Online Dating Website"*
655 +**DOI:** [https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/sow007](https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/sow007)
656 +**Subject Matter:** *Race and Dating, Multiracial Identity, Online Behavior*
657 +
658 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
659 +1. **General Observations:**
660 + - Data drawn from **over 1 million messaging records** from an online dating site.
661 + - Focused on how **monoracial users** (especially Whites) interact with **multiracial daters**.
662 +
663 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
664 + - **Multiracial Black/White and Asian/White women** received **fewer responses from White men** than their monoracial counterparts.
665 + - White daters showed **stronger preferences for monoracial identities**, particularly **own-race pairings**.
666 +
667 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
668 + - **Multiracial men** fared worse than multiracial women across most pairings.
669 + - **Latina/White and Asian/White multiracial women** were **more positively received by Black and Hispanic men**.
670 +{{/expandable}}
671 +
672 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
673 +1. **Primary Observations:**
674 + - White users demonstrated a clear pattern of **in-group preference**, preferring other White users (monoracial or partially White) over more ambiguous multiracial identities.
675 + - Authors suggest this reflects **"boundary-maintaining behavior"** and **"latent racial bias"**.
676 +
677 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
678 + - **Multiracial women with partial minority backgrounds** were more acceptable to non-White men than White men.
679 + - Multiracial daters were **often treated as ambiguous or “less desirable”** in ways the authors frame as **resistance to racial integration**.
680 +
681 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
682 + - The most rejected group? **Black/White multiracial men**, especially by **White women**, which the authors do not frame as bias in the same way.
683 + - The study shows **asymmetrical concern** — when Whites select inwardly, it's seen as racial boundary policing; when minorities do it, it's not pathologized.
684 +{{/expandable}}
685 +
686 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
687 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
688 + - Large, real-world dataset gives useful behavioral insight into **racial preferences in dating**.
689 + - Raises legitimate questions about **how race, desire, and group identity intersect**.
690 +
691 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
692 + - Frames **normal in-group preference among Whites as "resistance to multiraciality"**, rather than neutral human patterning.
693 + - Ignores **similar or stronger in-group preference among Black and Asian users**, which could indicate *universal patterns*, not White exceptionalism.
694 + - Uses CRT framing to subtly **morally indict Whites for preferring Whites**, while exempting other groups.
695 +
696 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
697 + - Treat all in-group preference equally across racial groups — not just when Whites do it.
698 + - Disaggregate by age, education, and regional variation to control for confounds.
699 + - Consider whether **multiracial identity is ambiguous** by nature and if that ambiguity reduces clarity of signals in dating.
700 +{{/expandable}}
701 +
702 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
703 +- Provides a data point in the **ongoing academic effort to pathologize White selectiveness**, even in private, personal domains like dating.
704 +- Demonstrates how **racial preferences are only considered “problematic” when they preserve White group boundaries**.
705 +- Supports analysis of **how DEI-aligned narratives seek to dissolve in-group loyalty under the guise of openness and inclusion**.
706 +{{/expandable}}
707 +
708 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
709 +1. Investigate how **media and dating platforms reinforce multiracialism as normative** despite evidence of natural in-group selection.
710 +2. Study the **psychological effects of being told your preferences are morally wrong if you're White**.
711 +3. Explore how **multiracial identities are strategically framed** depending on political or cultural goals — exoticization, integration, or guilt projection.
712 +{{/expandable}}
713 +
714 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
715 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:Curington et al. - Positioning Multiraciality in Cyberspace Treatment of Multiracial Daters in an Online Dating Websit.pdf]]
716 +{{/expandable}}
717 +{{/expandable}}
718 +
719 +
720 +{{expandable summary="Study: The White Man’s Burden: Gonzo Pornography and the Construction of Black Masculinity"}}
721 +**Source:** *Porn Studies*
722 +**Date of Publication:** *2015*
723 +**Author(s):** *Noah Tsika*
724 +**Title:** *"The White Man’s Burden: Gonzo Pornography and the Construction of Black Masculinity"*
725 +**DOI:** [10.1080/23268743.2015.1025389](https://doi.org/10.1080/23268743.2015.1025389)
726 +**Subject Matter:** *Pornography Studies, Race and Sexuality, Cultural Critique*
727 +
728 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
729 +1. **General Observations:**
730 + - This is a **qualitative content analysis** of gonzo pornography, particularly interracial porn involving Black men and White women.
731 + - The author reviews **select films, not a dataset**, using them to extrapolate broad cultural claims about race and sexuality.
732 +
733 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
734 + - Claims that **interracial porn “others” and dehumanizes Black men**, yet selectively **frames Black male sexual aggression as liberatory**.
735 + - The author accuses White male consumers of **fetishizing Black men** as both threats and tools for their own “colonial guilt.”
736 +
737 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
738 + - No empirical evidence, just interpretive readings of scenes and film dialogue.
739 + - Repeatedly criticizes **White directors and actors** as complicit in perpetuating “White supremacy through porn.”
740 +{{/expandable}}
741 +
742 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
743 +1. **Primary Observations:**
744 + - Argues that **gonzo interracial porn functions as racial propaganda**, reinforcing White guilt while commodifying Black masculinity.
745 + - Portrays White women as willing participants in a fantasy of racial domination that allegedly “liberates” Black men.
746 +
747 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
748 + - White male viewers are pathologized as both sexually repressed and voyeuristically complicit in anti-Black racism.
749 + - Black male performers are framed as both victims of racial commodification and **agents of resistance through hypersexuality**.
750 +
751 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
752 + - Cites scenes where Black male actors degrade or dominate White women as **“transgressive acts” that destabilize White power**, rather than examples of racial hostility or objectification.
753 + - The narrative treats **racially charged sexual violence as deconstructive**, only when it reverses traditional racial dynamics.
754 +{{/expandable}}
755 +
756 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
757 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
758 + - Useful in showcasing how **critical race theory invades even the most apolitical domains** (porn consumption) and turns them into race war battlegrounds.
759 + - Offers insight into how **White heterosexuality is recoded as colonialism** in activist academia.
760 +
761 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
762 + - **No statistical basis**, relies entirely on biased interpretive analysis of fringe media.
763 + - Presumes **intent and audience motivation** without surveys, viewership data, or cross-cultural comparison.
764 + - Treats Black aggression as empowering and White sexuality as inherently oppressive — a double standard.
765 +
766 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
767 + - Include comparative data on how different racial groups are portrayed in pornography across genres.
768 + - Analyze how **minority-run porn studios frame interracial themes** — not just White-directed media.
769 + - Address how racial fetishization **harms all groups**, not just Black men.
770 +{{/expandable}}
771 +
772 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
773 +- Exemplifies how **racialized sexual narratives are reinterpreted to indict White identity**, even in consumer entertainment.
774 +- Shows how **DEI and CRT frameworks are applied to pornographic material** to pathologize White maleness while sanctifying non-White hypermasculinity.
775 +- Highlights the **academic bias that treats transgressive content as empowering when it serves anti-White narratives**.
776 +{{/expandable}}
777 +
778 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
779 +1. Study how **interracial porn narratives differ when produced by non-White vs. White directors**.
780 +2. Examine **how racial power is portrayed in same-sex vs. heterosexual interracial porn**.
781 +3. Investigate whether the **fetishization of Black masculinity fuels unrealistic expectations and destructive stereotypes** for both Black and White men.
782 +{{/expandable}}
783 +
784 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
785 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:Dinest - The White Man's Burden Gonzo Pornography and the Construction of Black Masculinity.pdf]]
786 +{{/expandable}}
787 +{{/expandable}}
788 +
789 +
790 +{{expandable summary="Study: Gendered Racial Exclusion Among White Internet Daters"}}
791 +**Source:** *Social Science Research*
792 +**Date of Publication:** *2009*
793 +**Author(s):** *Cynthia Feliciano, Belinda Robnett, Golnaz Komaie*
794 +**Title:** *"Gendered Racial Exclusion Among White Internet Daters"*
795 +**DOI:** [10.1016/j.ssresearch.2009.04.004](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2009.04.004)
796 +**Subject Matter:** *Online Dating, Racial Preferences, CRT Framing of White Intimacy*
797 +
798 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
799 +1. **General Observations:**
800 + - Based on data from **Love@aol.com**, analyzing **over 6,000 profiles** from California.
801 + - The study investigated **racial preferences listed explicitly** in dating profiles.
802 +
803 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
804 + - **White women were least likely to express openness to interracial dating**, particularly with Black and Asian men.
805 + - **White men also showed exclusion**, but were more open than White women.
806 +
807 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
808 + - The authors labeled preference for one’s own race as **“racial exclusion”**.
809 + - Profiles by non-White users expressing same-race preferences were **not similarly problematized**.
810 +{{/expandable}}
811 +
812 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
813 +1. **Primary Observations:**
814 + - **White in-group preference was framed as discriminatory**, regardless of intent or context.
815 + - Dating preferences were interpreted as a **“reinforcement of racial hierarchies”**.
816 +
817 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
818 + - The study suggested **White women’s selectivity** stemmed from **cultural and structural advantages**, implying racial gatekeeping.
819 + - Did not critically examine **non-White preferences** for their own race.
820 +
821 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
822 + - Highlighted that **Latina and Asian women were more open to White men** than to men of their own ethnicity, which was not treated as exclusionary.
823 + - **No racial preference was criticized except when it protected White boundaries.**
824 +{{/expandable}}
825 +
826 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
827 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
828 + - Large dataset from real-world dating profiles.
829 + - Provides rare insight into **gendered patterns of racial preference**.
830 +
831 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
832 + - **Frames personal preference as political discrimination** when expressed by White users.
833 + - **Fails to control for cultural compatibility, attraction patterns, or religious values.**
834 + - **Double standard** in analysis — **non-White selectivity is ignored or justified.**
835 +
836 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
837 + - Should distinguish **racial animus from in-group preference**.
838 + - Include **psychological, aesthetic, and cultural compatibility data**.
839 + - Apply **equal critical lens to all racial groups**, not just Whites.
840 +{{/expandable}}
841 +
842 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
843 +- Reinforces how CRT-aligned research pathologizes **White in-group dating preferences**.
844 +- Supports the claim that **White intimacy boundaries are uniquely scrutinized** and politicized.
845 +- Demonstrates how even non-political behavior (e.g., dating) is racialized when it involves Whites.
846 +{{/expandable}}
847 +
848 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
849 +1. Study how **dating preferences vary by upbringing, media influence, and culture**, not just race.
850 +2. Analyze **racial preferences across all groups** with equal rigor and skepticism.
851 +3. Examine the **mental health impact of stigmatizing in-group preference** among Whites.
852 +{{/expandable}}
853 +
854 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
855 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.ssresearch.2009.04.004.pdf]]
856 +{{/expandable}}
857 +{{/expandable}}
858 +
859 +
860 +{{expandable summary="Study: Black Penis and the Demoralization of the Western World"}}
861 +**Source:** *Journal of European Psychoanalysis*
862 +**Date of Publication:** *2009*
863 +**Author(s):** *Kristen Fink* *Jewish*))
864 +**Title:** *"Black Penis and the Demoralization of the Western World: Sexual relationships between black men and white women as a cause of decline"*
865 +**DOI:** *Unavailable – Psychoanalytic essay publication*
866 +**Subject Matter:** *Race and Sexuality, Psychoanalysis, Cultural Demoralization*
867 +
868 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
869 +1. **General Observations:**
870 + - This is a **psychoanalytic essay**, not an empirical study.
871 + - Uses **Freudian and Lacanian theory** to explore symbolic meanings of interracial sex.
872 + - Frames **Black male–White female pairings** as psychologically disruptive to the White male ego and Western civilization.
873 +
874 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
875 + - Positions **Black men as symbolic rivals** to emasculated Western (White) men.
876 + - **White women’s interracial attraction** is framed as rebellion or rejection of Western order.
877 +
878 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
879 + - The essay proposes that **sexual representation in media** is demoralizing to White culture.
880 + - Uses **high theory language** to justify what is ultimately an anti-White cultural narrative.
881 +{{/expandable}}
882 +
883 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
884 +1. **Primary Observations:**
885 + - **Interracial sexual dynamics** are framed as central to **Western decline**.
886 + - **White masculinity is portrayed as passive, obsolete, or neurotic** in contrast to hypermasculinized Blackness.
887 +
888 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
889 + - Suggests White men internalize emasculation through exposure to interracial symbolism.
890 + - Sees **cultural loss of confidence** in White society as stemming from racial-sexual symbolism.
891 +
892 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
893 + - Analyzes media tropes (e.g., interracial porn, pop culture) through the lens of psychoanalytic guilt and transgression.
894 + - Never critiques the **ideological project of glorifying Blackness at the expense of White identity**.
895 +{{/expandable}}
896 +
897 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
898 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
899 + - Reveals how **elite academic disciplines like psychoanalysis** are used to mask anti-White narratives in esoteric jargon.
900 + - Serves as **ideological evidence** of demoralization tactics embedded in cultural theory.
901 +
902 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
903 + - No empirical data, surveys, or statistical analysis — purely speculative.
904 + - **Does not critique hypersexualization of Black men** or the dehumanizing aspects of the fetish.
905 + - Assumes **White masculinity must passively accept its symbolic erasure** as psychoanalytically “natural.”
906 +
907 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
908 + - Include **perspectives from White men and women** on how these portrayals affect their psychological well-being.
909 + - Disentangle psychoanalytic theory from **racial guilt ideology**.
910 + - Explore **mutual respect-based frameworks** for interracial dynamics rather than ones rooted in humiliation or power symbolism.
911 +{{/expandable}}
912 +
913 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
914 +- Illustrates how **race, sex, and culture are manipulated to undermine White self-perception**.
915 +- Demonstrates how **academic elites frame White decline as psychologically necessary or deserved**.
916 +- Provides ideological background for modern media trends that eroticize racial power imbalance.
917 +{{/expandable}}
918 +
919 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
920 +1. Analyze how psychoanalytic language is used to **justify racial inversion in cultural dominance**.
921 +2. Examine the **role of pornography in demoralization campaigns** targeting White men.
922 +3. Explore how elite journals create **ideological cover for overt anti-White sentiment**.
923 +{{/expandable}}
924 +
925 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
926 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.Fink_Black_Penis_Demoralization.pdf]]
927 +{{/expandable}}
928 +{{/expandable}}
929 +
930 +
931 +{{expandable summary="Study: Trends in Frequency of Sexual Activity and Number of Sexual Partners Among Adults Aged 18 to 44 Years in the US, 2000-2018"}}
932 +**Source:** *JAMA Network Open*
933 +**Date of Publication:** *2020*
934 +**Author(s):** *Ueda P, Mercer CH, Ghaznavi C, Herbenick D.*
935 +**Title:** *"Trends in Frequency of Sexual Activity and Number of Sexual Partners Among Adults Aged 18 to 44 Years in the US, 2000-2018"*
936 +**DOI:** [10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3833](https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3833)
937 +**Subject Matter:** *Public Health, Sexual Behavior, Demography* 
938 +
939 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
940 +1. **General Observations:**
941 + - Study analyzed **General Social Survey (2000-2018)** data.
942 + - Found **declining trends in sexual activity** among young adults.
943 +
944 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
945 + - Decreases in sexual activity were most prominent among **men aged 18-34**.
946 + - Factors like **marital status, employment, and psychological well-being** were associated with changes in sexual frequency.
947 +
948 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
949 + - Frequency of sexual activity decreased by **8-10%** over the studied period.
950 + - Number of sexual partners remained **relatively stable** despite declining activity rates.
951 +{{/expandable}}
952 +
953 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
954 +1. **Primary Observations:**
955 + - A significant decline in sexual frequency, especially among **younger men**.
956 + - Shifts in relationship dynamics and economic stressors may contribute to the trend.
957 +
958 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
959 + - More pronounced decline among **unmarried individuals**.
960 + - No major change observed for **married adults** over time.
961 +
962 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
963 + - **Mental health and employment status** were correlated with decreased activity.
964 + - Social factors such as **screen time and digital entertainment consumption** are potential contributors.
965 +{{/expandable}}
966 +
967 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
968 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
969 + - **Large sample size** from a nationally representative dataset.
970 + - **Longitudinal design** enables trend analysis over time.
971 +
972 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
973 + - Self-reported data may introduce **response bias**.
974 + - No direct causal mechanisms tested for the decline in sexual activity.
975 +
976 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
977 + - Further studies should incorporate **qualitative data** on behavioral shifts.
978 + - Additional factors such as **economic shifts and social media usage** need exploration.
979 +{{/expandable}}
980 +
981 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
982 +- Provides evidence on **changing demographic behaviors** in relation to relationships and social interactions.
983 +- Highlights the role of **mental health, employment, and societal changes** in personal behaviors.
984 +{{/expandable}}
985 +
986 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
987 +1. Investigate the **impact of digital media consumption** on relationship dynamics.
988 +2. Examine **regional and cultural differences** in sexual activity trends.
989 +{{/expandable}}
990 +
991 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
992 +
993 +{{/expandable}}
994 +{{/expandable}}
995 +
996 +{{expandable summary="Study: Biracial Couples and Adverse Birth Outcomes – A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis"}}
997 +**Source:** *Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica*
998 +**Date of Publication:** *2012*
999 +**Author(s):** *Ravisha M. Srinivasjois, Shreya Shah, Prakesh S. Shah, Knowledge Synthesis Group on Determinants of Preterm/LBW Births*
1000 +**Title:** *"Biracial Couples and Adverse Birth Outcomes: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis"*
1001 +**DOI:** [10.1111/j.1600-0412.2012.01501.x](https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0412.2012.01501.x)
1002 +**Subject Matter:** *Neonatal Health, Maternal-Fetal Medicine, Racial Disparities*
1003 +
1004 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1005 +1. **General Observations:**
1006 + - Meta-analysis of **26,335,596 singleton births** from eight studies.
1007 + - **Higher risk of adverse birth outcomes in biracial couples** than White couples, but lower than Black couples.
1008 +
1009 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1010 + - **Maternal race had a stronger influence than paternal race** on birth outcomes.
1011 + - **Black mother–White father (BMWF) couples** had a higher risk than **White mother–Black father (WMBF) couples**.
1012 +
1013 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1014 + - **Adjusted Odds Ratios (aORs) for key outcomes:**
1015 + - **Low birthweight (LBW):** WMBF (1.21), BMWF (1.75), Black mother–Black father (BMBF) (2.08).
1016 + - **Preterm births (PTB):** WMBF (1.17), BMWF (1.37), BMBF (1.78).
1017 + - **Stillbirths:** WMBF (1.43), BMWF (1.51), BMBF (1.85).
1018 +{{/expandable}}
1019 +
1020 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1021 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1022 + - **Biracial couples face a gradient of risk**: higher than White couples but lower than Black couples.
1023 + - **Maternal race plays a more significant role** in pregnancy outcomes.
1024 +
1025 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1026 + - **Black mothers (regardless of paternal race) had the highest risk of LBW and PTB**.
1027 + - **White mothers with Black fathers had a lower risk** than Black mothers with White fathers.
1028 +
1029 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1030 + - The **weathering hypothesis** suggests that **long-term stress exposure** contributes to higher adverse birth risks in Black mothers.
1031 + - **Genetic and environmental factors** may interact to influence birth outcomes.
1032 +{{/expandable}}
1033 +
1034 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1035 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1036 + - **Largest meta-analysis** on racial disparities in birth outcomes.
1037 + - Uses **adjusted statistical models** to account for confounding variables.
1038 +
1039 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1040 + - Data limited to **Black-White biracial couples**, excluding other racial groups.
1041 + - **Socioeconomic and healthcare access factors** not fully explored.
1042 +
1043 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1044 + - Future studies should examine **Asian, Hispanic, and Indigenous biracial couples**.
1045 + - Investigate **long-term health effects on infants from biracial pregnancies**.
1046 +{{/expandable}}
1047 +
1048 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1049 +- Provides **critical insights into racial disparities** in maternal and infant health.
1050 +- Supports **research on genetic and environmental influences on neonatal health**.
1051 +- Highlights **how maternal race plays a more significant role than paternal race** in birth outcomes.
1052 +{{/expandable}}
1053 +
1054 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1055 +1. Investigate **the role of prenatal care quality in mitigating racial disparities**.
1056 +2. Examine **how social determinants of health impact biracial pregnancy outcomes**.
1057 +3. Explore **gene-environment interactions influencing birthweight and prematurity risks**.
1058 +{{/expandable}}
1059 +
1060 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1061 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1111_j.1600-0412.2012.01501.xAbstract.pdf]]
1062 +{{/expandable}}
1063 +{{/expandable}}
1064 +
1065 +{{expandable summary="Study: One is the Loneliest Number: Involuntary Celibacy (Incel), Mental Health, and Loneliness"}}
1066 +**Source:** *Current Psychology*
1067 +**Date of Publication:** *2024*
1068 +**Author(s):** *Brandon Sparks, Alexandra M. Zidenberg, Mark E. Olver*
1069 +**Title:** *"One is the Loneliest Number: Involuntary Celibacy (Incel), Mental Health, and Loneliness"*
1070 +**DOI:** [10.1007/s12144-023-04275-z](https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04275-z)
1071 +**Subject Matter:** *Psychology, Mental Health, Social Isolation*
1072 +
1073 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1074 +1. **General Observations:**
1075 + - Study analyzed **67 self-identified incels** and **103 non-incel men**.
1076 + - Incels reported **higher loneliness and lower social support** compared to non-incels.
1077 +
1078 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1079 + - Incels exhibited **higher levels of depression, anxiety, and self-critical rumination**.
1080 + - **Social isolation was a key factor** differentiating incels from non-incels.
1081 +
1082 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1083 + - 95% of incels in the study reported **having depression**, with 38% receiving a formal diagnosis.
1084 + - **Higher externalization of blame** was linked to stronger incel identification.
1085 +{{/expandable}}
1086 +
1087 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1088 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1089 + - Incels experience **heightened rejection sensitivity and loneliness**.
1090 + - Lack of social support correlates with **worse mental health outcomes**.
1091 +
1092 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1093 + - **Avoidant attachment styles** were a strong predictor of incel identity.
1094 + - **Mate value perceptions** significantly differed between incels and non-incels.
1095 +
1096 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1097 + - Incels **engaged in fewer positive coping mechanisms** such as emotional support or positive reframing.
1098 + - Instead, they relied on **solitary coping strategies**, worsening their isolation.
1099 +{{/expandable}}
1100 +
1101 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1102 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1103 + - **First quantitative study** on incels’ social isolation and mental health.
1104 + - **Robust sample size** and validated psychological measures.
1105 +
1106 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1107 + - Sample drawn from **Reddit communities**, which may not represent all incels.
1108 + - **No causal conclusions**—correlations between isolation and inceldom need further research.
1109 +
1110 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1111 + - Future studies should **compare incel forum users vs. non-users**.
1112 + - Investigate **potential intervention strategies** for social integration.
1113 +{{/expandable}}
1114 +
1115 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1116 +- Highlights **mental health vulnerabilities** within the incel community.
1117 +- Supports research on **loneliness, attachment styles, and social dominance orientation**.
1118 +- Examines how **peer rejection influences self-perceived mate value**.
1119 +{{/expandable}}
1120 +
1121 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1122 +1. Explore how **online community participation** affects incel mental health.
1123 +2. Investigate **cognitive biases** influencing self-perceived rejection among incels.
1124 +3. Assess **therapeutic interventions** to address incel social isolation.
1125 +{{/expandable}}
1126 +
1127 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1128 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1007_s12144-023-04275-z.pdf]]
1129 +{{/expandable}}
1130 +{{/expandable}}
1131 +
1132 += Crime and Substance Abuse =
1133 +
1134 +{{expandable summary="Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"}}
1135 +**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse*
1136 +**Date of Publication:** *2002*
1137 +**Author(s):** *Clifford A. Butzin, Christine A. Saum, Frank R. Scarpitti*
1138 +**Title:** *"Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"*
1139 +**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120014424](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120014424)
1140 +**Subject Matter:** *Substance Use, Criminal Justice, Drug Courts*
1141 +
1142 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1143 +1. **General Observations:**
1144 + - Study examined **drug treatment court success rates** among first-time offenders.
1145 + - Strongest predictors of **successful completion were employment status and race**.
1146 +
1147 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1148 + - Individuals with **stable jobs were more likely to complete the program**.
1149 + - **Black participants had lower success rates**, suggesting potential systemic disparities.
1150 +
1151 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1152 + - **Education level was positively correlated** with program completion.
1153 + - Frequency of **drug use before enrollment affected treatment outcomes**.
1154 +{{/expandable}}
1155 +
1156 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1157 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1158 + - **Social stability factors** (employment, education) were key to treatment success.
1159 + - **Race and pre-existing substance use patterns** influenced completion rates.
1160 +
1161 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1162 + - White offenders had **higher completion rates** than Black offenders.
1163 + - Drug court success was **higher for those with lower initial drug use frequency**.
1164 +
1165 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1166 + - **Individuals with strong social ties were more likely to finish the program**.
1167 + - Success rates were **significantly higher for participants with case management support**.
1168 +{{/expandable}}
1169 +
1170 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1171 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1172 + - **First empirical study on drug court program success factors**.
1173 + - Uses **longitudinal data** for post-treatment analysis.
1174 +
1175 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1176 + - Lacks **qualitative data on personal motivation and treatment engagement**.
1177 + - Focuses on **short-term program success** without tracking **long-term relapse rates**.
1178 +
1179 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1180 + - Future research should examine **racial disparities in drug court outcomes**.
1181 + - Study **how community resources impact long-term recovery**.
1182 +{{/expandable}}
1183 +
1184 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1185 +- Provides insight into **what factors contribute to drug court program success**.
1186 +- Highlights **racial disparities in criminal justice-based rehabilitation programs**.
1187 +- Supports **policy discussions on improving access to drug treatment for marginalized groups**.
1188 +{{/expandable}}
1189 +
1190 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1191 +1. Investigate **the role of mental health in drug court success rates**.
1192 +2. Assess **long-term relapse prevention strategies post-treatment**.
1193 +3. Explore **alternative diversion programs beyond traditional drug courts**.
1194 +{{/expandable}}
1195 +
1196 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1197 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120014424.pdf]]
1198 +{{/expandable}}
1199 +{{/expandable}}
1200 +
1201 +{{expandable summary="Study: Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys"}}
1202 +**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse*
1203 +**Date of Publication:** *2003*
1204 +**Author(s):** *Timothy P. Johnson, Phillip J. Bowman*
1205 +**Title:** *"Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys"*
1206 +**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120023394](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120023394)
1207 +**Subject Matter:** *Survey Methodology, Racial Disparities, Substance Use Research*
1208 +
1209 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1210 +1. **General Observations:**
1211 + - Study examined **how racial and cultural factors influence self-reported substance use data**.
1212 + - Analyzed **36 empirical studies from 1977–2003** on survey reliability across racial/ethnic groups.
1213 +
1214 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1215 + - Black and Latino respondents **were more likely to underreport drug use** compared to White respondents.
1216 + - **Cultural stigma and distrust in research institutions** affected self-report accuracy.
1217 +
1218 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1219 + - **Surveys using biological validation (urinalysis, hair tests) revealed underreporting trends**.
1220 + - **Higher recantation rates** (denying past drug use) were observed among minority respondents.
1221 +{{/expandable}}
1222 +
1223 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1224 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1225 + - Racial/ethnic disparities in **substance use reporting bias survey-based research**.
1226 + - **Social desirability and cultural norms impact data reliability**.
1227 +
1228 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1229 + - White respondents were **more likely to overreport** substance use.
1230 + - Black and Latino respondents **had higher recantation rates**, particularly in face-to-face interviews.
1231 +
1232 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1233 + - Mode of survey administration **significantly influenced reporting accuracy**.
1234 + - **Self-administered surveys produced more reliable data than interviewer-administered surveys**.
1235 +{{/expandable}}
1236 +
1237 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1238 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1239 + - **Comprehensive review of 36 studies** on measurement error in substance use reporting.
1240 + - Identifies **systemic biases affecting racial/ethnic survey reliability**.
1241 +
1242 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1243 + - Relies on **secondary data analysis**, limiting direct experimental control.
1244 + - Does not explore **how measurement error impacts policy decisions**.
1245 +
1246 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1247 + - Future research should **incorporate mixed-method approaches** (qualitative & quantitative).
1248 + - Investigate **how survey design can reduce racial reporting disparities**.
1249 +{{/expandable}}
1250 +
1251 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1252 +- Supports research on **racial disparities in self-reported health behaviors**.
1253 +- Highlights **survey methodology issues that impact substance use epidemiology**.
1254 +- Provides insights for **improving data accuracy in public health research**.
1255 +{{/expandable}}
1256 +
1257 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1258 +1. Investigate **how survey design impacts racial disparities in self-reported health data**.
1259 +2. Study **alternative data collection methods (biometric validation, passive data tracking)**.
1260 +3. Explore **the role of social stigma in self-reported health behaviors**.
1261 +{{/expandable}}
1262 +
1263 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1264 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120023394.pdf]]
1265 +{{/expandable}}
1266 +{{/expandable}}
1267 +
1268 +{{expandable summary="Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"}}
1269 +**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse*
1270 +**Date of Publication:** *2002*
1271 +**Author(s):** *Clifford A. Butzin, Christine A. Saum, Frank R. Scarpitti*
1272 +**Title:** *"Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"*
1273 +**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120014424](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120014424)
1274 +**Subject Matter:** *Substance Use, Criminal Justice, Drug Courts*
1275 +
1276 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1277 +1. **General Observations:**
1278 + - Study examined **drug treatment court success rates** among first-time offenders.
1279 + - Strongest predictors of **successful completion were employment status and race**.
1280 +
1281 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1282 + - Individuals with **stable jobs were more likely to complete the program**.
1283 + - **Black participants had lower success rates**, suggesting potential systemic disparities.
1284 +
1285 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1286 + - **Education level was positively correlated** with program completion.
1287 + - Frequency of **drug use before enrollment affected treatment outcomes**.
1288 +{{/expandable}}
1289 +
1290 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1291 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1292 + - **Social stability factors** (employment, education) were key to treatment success.
1293 + - **Race and pre-existing substance use patterns** influenced completion rates.
1294 +
1295 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1296 + - White offenders had **higher completion rates** than Black offenders.
1297 + - Drug court success was **higher for those with lower initial drug use frequency**.
1298 +
1299 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1300 + - **Individuals with strong social ties were more likely to finish the program**.
1301 + - Success rates were **significantly higher for participants with case management support**.
1302 +{{/expandable}}
1303 +
1304 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1305 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1306 + - **First empirical study on drug court program success factors**.
1307 + - Uses **longitudinal data** for post-treatment analysis.
1308 +
1309 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1310 + - Lacks **qualitative data on personal motivation and treatment engagement**.
1311 + - Focuses on **short-term program success** without tracking **long-term relapse rates**.
1312 +
1313 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1314 + - Future research should examine **racial disparities in drug court outcomes**.
1315 + - Study **how community resources impact long-term recovery**.
1316 +{{/expandable}}
1317 +
1318 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1319 +- Provides insight into **what factors contribute to drug court program success**.
1320 +- Highlights **racial disparities in criminal justice-based rehabilitation programs**.
1321 +- Supports **policy discussions on improving access to drug treatment for marginalized groups**.
1322 +{{/expandable}}
1323 +
1324 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1325 +1. Investigate **the role of mental health in drug court success rates**.
1326 +2. Assess **long-term relapse prevention strategies post-treatment**.
1327 +3. Explore **alternative diversion programs beyond traditional drug courts**.
1328 +{{/expandable}}
1329 +
1330 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1331 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120014424.pdf]]
1332 +{{/expandable}}
1333 +{{/expandable}}
1334 +
1335 +{{expandable summary="
1336 +
1337 +Study: Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time?"}}
1338 +**Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)*
1339 +**Date of Publication:** *2014*
1340 +**Author(s):** *Michael A. Woodley, Jan te Nijenhuis, Raegan Murphy*
1341 +**Title:** *"Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time?"*
1342 +**DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.012](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.012)
1343 +**Subject Matter:** *Cognitive Decline, Intelligence, Dysgenics*
1344 +
1345 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1346 +1. **General Observations:**
1347 + - The study examines reaction time data from **13 age-matched studies** spanning **1884–2004**.
1348 + - Results suggest an estimated **decline of 13.35 IQ points** over this period.
1349 +
1350 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1351 + - The study found **slower reaction times in modern populations** compared to Victorian-era individuals.
1352 + - Data from **Western countries (US, UK, Canada, Australia, Finland)** were analyzed.
1353 +
1354 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1355 + - The estimated **dysgenic rate is 1.21 IQ points lost per decade**.
1356 + - Meta-regression analysis confirmed a **steady secular trend in slowing reaction time**.
1357 +{{/expandable}}
1358 +
1359 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1360 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1361 + - Supports the hypothesis of **intelligence decline due to genetic and environmental factors**.
1362 + - Reaction time, a **biomarker for cognitive ability**, has slowed significantly over time.
1363 +
1364 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1365 + - A stronger **correlation between slower reaction time and lower general intelligence (g)**.
1366 + - Flynn effect (IQ gains) does not contradict this finding, as reaction time is a **biological, not environmental, measure**.
1367 +
1368 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1369 + - Cross-national comparisons indicate a **global trend in slower reaction times**.
1370 + - Factors like **modern neurotoxin exposure** and **reduced selective pressure for intelligence** may contribute.
1371 +{{/expandable}}
1372 +
1373 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1374 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1375 + - **Comprehensive meta-analysis** covering over a century of reaction time data.
1376 + - **Robust statistical corrections** for measurement variance between historical and modern studies.
1377 +
1378 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1379 + - Some historical data sources **lack methodological consistency**.
1380 + - **Reaction time measurements vary by study**, requiring adjustments for equipment differences.
1381 +
1382 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1383 + - Future studies should **replicate results with more modern datasets**.
1384 + - Investigate **alternative cognitive biomarkers** for intelligence over time.
1385 +{{/expandable}}
1386 +
1387 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1388 +- Provides evidence for **long-term intelligence trends**, contributing to research on **cognitive evolution**.
1389 +- Aligns with broader discussions on **dysgenics, neurophysiology, and cognitive load**.
1390 +- Supports the argument that **modern societies may be experiencing intelligence decline**.
1391 +{{/expandable}}
1392 +
1393 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1394 +1. Investigate **genetic markers associated with reaction time** and intelligence decline.
1395 +2. Examine **regional variations in reaction time trends**.
1396 +3. Explore **cognitive resilience factors that counteract the decline**.
1397 +{{/expandable}}
1398 +
1399 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1400 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2014.05.012.pdf]]
1401 +{{/expandable}}
1402 +{{/expandable}}
1403 +
1404 += Whiteness & White Guilt =
1405 +
1406 +{{expandable summary="Study: Reducing Implicit Racial Preferences: I. A Comparative Investigation of 17 Interventions"}}
1407 +**Source:** *Psychological Science*
1408 +**Date of Publication:** *2014*
1409 +**Author(s):** *Caleb E. Lai, Anthony G. Greenwald, et al.*
1410 +**Title:** *"Reducing Implicit Racial Preferences: I. A Comparative Investigation of 17 Interventions"*
1411 +**DOI:** [10.1177/0956797614535812](https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614535812)
1412 +**Subject Matter:** *Implicit Bias, Racial Psychology, Psychological Conditioning*
1413 +
1414 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1415 +1. **General Observations:**
1416 + - Tested **17 different interventions** across **6,321 participants**, all measured via IAT (Implicit Association Test).
1417 + - Focused exclusively on reducing **pro-White, anti-Black preferences** — no reciprocal testing on anti-White bias.
1418 +
1419 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1420 + - Educational and exposure-based interventions (e.g., multiculturalism, egalitarian messaging) failed to reduce bias significantly.
1421 + - Most effective short-term results came from **trauma-based or emotionally coercive interventions**.
1422 +
1423 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1424 + - The **"Black hero" intervention**, where participants imagined being violently attacked by a White man and rescued by a Black man, was among the most effective.
1425 + - Effects of even the most extreme interventions **dissipated within 24–72 hours**, with no long-term behavioral change.
1426 +{{/expandable}}
1427 +
1428 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1429 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1430 + - The interventions that produced the most dramatic IAT changes used **emotionally graphic narratives** depicting Whites as violent aggressors and Blacks as saviors.
1431 + - Merely showing positive Black images or promoting egalitarian values had minimal effect on implicit associations.
1432 +
1433 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1434 + - In the **"Black hero" condition**, participants were asked to imagine being physically beaten by a White person and then rescued by a Black person — an intentionally vivid and disturbing scenario.
1435 + - The **"Black victim" intervention** relied on emotionally shocking imagery of anti-Black violence (e.g., lynching) to induce guilt and disrupt positive associations with Whiteness.
1436 +
1437 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1438 + - None of the scenarios reversed the framing (e.g., Black aggressor/White victim), confirming the ideological goal was **to degrade White identity**, not merely reduce bias.
1439 + - The study was **cited by the White House Office of Science and Technology Policy (OSTP)** to justify DEI-aligned policy recommendations.
1440 +{{/expandable}}
1441 +
1442 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1443 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1444 + - Large sample size and systematic comparison across diverse intervention types.
1445 + - Clearly shows that **implicit preference is resilient** and not easily changed by education or exposure alone.
1446 +
1447 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1448 + - The most “effective” methods **relied on emotional manipulation, not persuasion or evidence**.
1449 + - Assumes **natural in-group preference is pathological** when expressed by White subjects but makes no effort to test other groups.
1450 + - **Zero attention to pro-Black or anti-White bias** — only White attitudes are pathologized.
1451 +
1452 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1453 + - Test the **psychological harm** and ethical implications of using graphic racial trauma to coerce attitude change.
1454 + - Include interventions that **strengthen ingroup empathy** without demonizing other groups.
1455 + - Disaggregate bias by **class, region, and individual experience**, rather than racially reducing all bias to “Whiteness.”
1456 +{{/expandable}}
1457 +
1458 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1459 +- Provides direct evidence that **DEI-style implicit bias training** is based on emotionally abusive and **anti-White psychological framing**.
1460 +- Shows how **social science selectively targets Whites for attitude correction**, often using fictionalized racial trauma scenarios.
1461 +- Demonstrates that even extreme interventions **fail to achieve long-term change**, undermining the scientific justification for such policies.
1462 +{{/expandable}}
1463 +
1464 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1465 +1. Investigate **implicit bias training outcomes** in real-world institutional settings.
1466 +2. Study **the ethical limits of psychological reprogramming** in DEI policies.
1467 +3. Explore **natural ingroup preference across all races** using morally neutral frameworks.
1468 +{{/expandable}}
1469 +
1470 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1471 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:lai2014.pdf]]
1472 +{{/expandable}}
1473 +{{/expandable}}
1474 +
1475 +
1476 +{{expandable summary="Study: Segregation, Innocence, and Protection: The Institutional Conditions That Maintain Whiteness in College Sports"}}
1477 +**Source:** *Journal of Diversity in Higher Education*
1478 +**Date of Publication:** *2019*
1479 +**Author(s):** *Kirsten Hextrum*
1480 +**Title:** *"Segregation, Innocence, and Protection: The Institutional Conditions That Maintain Whiteness in College Sports"*
1481 +**DOI:** [10.1037/dhe0000140](https://doi.org/10.1037/dhe0000140)
1482 +**Subject Matter:** *Critical Race Theory, Sports Sociology, Anti-White Institutional Framing*
1483 +
1484 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1485 +1. **General Observations:**
1486 + - Based on **47 athlete interviews**, cherry-picked from non-revenue Division I sports.
1487 + - The study claims **“segregation”**, but presents no evidence of actual exclusion or policy bias — just demographic imbalance.
1488 +
1489 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1490 + - Attributes **White participation** in certain sports to "systemic racism", ignoring **self-selection, geography, and cultural affinity**.
1491 + - Claims White athletes are “protected” from race discussions — but never engages with **Black overrepresentation in revenue sports**.
1492 +
1493 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1494 + - White athletes are portrayed as **ignorant of their privilege**, a claim drawn entirely from CRT frameworks rather than behavior or outcome.
1495 + - **No empirical data** is offered on policy, scholarship distribution, or team selection criteria.
1496 +{{/expandable}}
1497 +
1498 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1499 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1500 + - Frames **normal demographic patterns** (e.g., majority-White rosters in tennis or rowing) as "institutional whiteness".
1501 + - **Ignores the structural dominance** of Black athletes in high-profile revenue sports like football and basketball.
1502 +
1503 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1504 + - White athletes are criticized for **lacking racial awareness**, reinforcing the moral framing of **Whiteness as inherently problematic**.
1505 + - **Cultural preference, individual merit, and athletic subculture** are all excluded from consideration.
1506 +
1507 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1508 + - Argues that college sports **reinforce racial hierarchy** without ever showing how White athletes benefit more than Black athletes.
1509 + - Offers **no comparative analysis** of scholarships, graduation rates, or media portrayal by race.
1510 +{{/expandable}}
1511 +
1512 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1513 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1514 + - Useful as a clear example of **how CRT ideologues weaponize demography** to frame White majority spaces as inherently suspect.
1515 + - Shows how **academic literature systematically avoids symmetrical analysis** when outcomes favor White participants.
1516 +
1517 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1518 + - **Excludes revenue sports**, where Black athletes dominate by numbers, prestige, and compensation.
1519 + - **Fails to explain** how team composition emerges from voluntary participation, geography, or subcultural identity.
1520 + - Treats **racial imbalance as proof of racism**, bypassing merit, interest, or socioeconomic context.
1521 +
1522 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1523 + - Include **White athlete perspectives** without pre-framing them as racially naive or complicit.
1524 + - **Compare all sports**, including those where Black athletes thrive and lead.
1525 + - Remove CRT framing and **evaluate outcomes empirically**, not ideologically.
1526 +{{/expandable}}
1527 +
1528 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1529 +- Demonstrates how **DEI-aligned research reframes benign patterns** as oppressive when White majorities are involved.
1530 +- Illustrates **anti-White academic framing** in environments where no institutional barrier exists.
1531 +- Provides a concrete example of how **CRT avoids acknowledging Black dominance in elite spaces** (revenue athletics).
1532 +{{/expandable}}
1533 +
1534 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1535 +1. Investigate **racial self-sorting and cultural affiliation** in athletic participation.
1536 +2. Compare **media framing of White-majority vs. Black-majority sports**.
1537 +3. Study **how CRT narratives distort athletic merit and demographic outcomes**.
1538 +{{/expandable}}
1539 +
1540 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1541 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1037_dhe0000140.pdf]]
1542 +{{/expandable}}
1543 +{{/expandable}}
1544 +
1545 +
1546 +{{expandable summary="Study: Racial Bias in Pain Assessment and Treatment Recommendations"}}
1547 +**Source:** *Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS)*
1548 +**Date of Publication:** *2016*
1549 +**Author(s):** *Kelly M. Hoffman, Sophie Trawalter, Jordan R. Axt, M. Norman Oliver*
1550 +**Title:** *"Racial Bias in Pain Assessment and Treatment Recommendations, and False Beliefs About Biological Differences Between Blacks and Whites"*
1551 +**DOI:** [10.1073/pnas.1516047113](https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1516047113)
1552 +**Subject Matter:** *Medical Ethics, Race in Medicine, Implicit Bias*
1553 +
1554 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1555 +1. **General Observations:**
1556 + - Analyzed responses from **222 white medical students and residents**.
1557 + - Investigated belief in **false biological differences between Black and White people**.
1558 + - Measured how those beliefs affected **pain ratings and treatment recommendations**.
1559 +
1560 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1561 + - **50% of participants endorsed at least one false belief** (e.g., Black people have thicker skin or less sensitive nerve endings).
1562 + - Those who endorsed false beliefs were **more likely to underestimate Black patients' pain**.
1563 +
1564 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1565 + - Bias was **most prominent among first-year students**, diminishing slightly with experience.
1566 + - Study used **hypothetical case vignettes**, not real patient data.
1567 +{{/expandable}}
1568 +
1569 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1570 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1571 + - False biological beliefs were **strongly correlated with racial disparity** in pain assessment.
1572 + - Endorsement of such beliefs led to **less appropriate treatment for Black patients** in fictional cases.
1573 +
1574 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1575 + - Medical students with **no false beliefs showed no treatment bias**.
1576 + - No evidence was presented of **active discrimination** — bias appeared linked to **misinformation, not malice**.
1577 +
1578 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1579 + - Fictional vignettes demonstrated that **misinformation about biology**, not systemic malice, led to unequal care.
1580 + - The study **did not show bias against White patients**, nor explore disparities affecting them.
1581 +{{/expandable}}
1582 +
1583 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1584 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1585 + - Provides valuable insight into **how medical myths can affect judgment**.
1586 + - Demonstrates the importance of **clinical education and evidence-based practice**.
1587 +
1588 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1589 + - Fails to examine **bias affecting White patients**, including under-treatment of opioid dependence or mental health.
1590 + - Only focuses on one direction of disparity, treating **White patients as a control** rather than a population worthy of study.
1591 + - **Overemphasizes "racial bias"** narrative despite the findings being more about **ignorance than intent**.
1592 +
1593 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1594 + - Include **comparison groups for all races**, not just a binary Black–White framework.
1595 + - Investigate **systemic neglect of poor rural White populations**, especially in Appalachia and the Midwest.
1596 + - Clarify the **distinction between false belief and racial animus**, which the study conflates under CRT framing.
1597 +{{/expandable}}
1598 +
1599 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1600 +- Shows how **DEI-aligned narratives exploit limited findings** to vilify White professionals.
1601 +- Provides an example of a **legitimate medical education issue being repackaged as “racial bias.”**
1602 +- Highlights the **lack of reciprocal scrutiny** of how minorities may receive **preferential narrative framing** or **programmatic support**.
1603 +{{/expandable}}
1604 +
1605 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1606 +1. Study whether **DEI training reduces false beliefs** or simply **induces White guilt**.
1607 +2. Investigate **biases against White rural patients**, especially regarding **opioid or pain management stigma**.
1608 +3. Conduct **clinical outcome studies**, not self-reported vignettes, to test **real-world disparities**.
1609 +{{/expandable}}
1610 +
1611 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1612 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1073_pnas.1516047113.pdf]]
1613 +{{/expandable}}
1614 +{{/expandable}}
1615 +
1616 +
1617 +{{expandable summary="Study: Rising Morbidity and Mortality in Midlife Among White Non-Hispanic Americans"}}
1618 +**Source:** *Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS)*
1619 +**Date of Publication:** *2015*
1620 +**Author(s):** *Anne Case, Angus Deaton*
1621 +**Title:** *"Rising Morbidity and Mortality in Midlife Among White Non-Hispanic Americans in the 21st Century"*
1622 +**DOI:** [10.1073/pnas.1518393112](https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1518393112)
1623 +**Subject Matter:** *Public Health, Mortality, Socioeconomic Factors*
1624 +
1625 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1626 +1. **General Observations:**
1627 + - Mortality rates among **middle-aged white non-Hispanic Americans (ages 45–54)** increased from 1999 to 2013.
1628 + - This reversal in mortality trends is unique to the U.S.; **no other wealthy country experienced a similar rise**.
1629 +
1630 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1631 + - The increase was **most pronounced among those with a high school education or less**.
1632 + - Hispanic and Black non-Hispanic mortality continued to decline over the same period.
1633 +
1634 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1635 + - Rising mortality was driven primarily by **suicide, drug and alcohol poisoning, and chronic liver disease**.
1636 + - Midlife morbidity increased as well, with more reports of **poor health, pain, and mental distress**.
1637 +{{/expandable}}
1638 +
1639 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1640 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1641 + - The rise in mortality is attributed to **substance abuse, economic distress, and deteriorating mental health**.
1642 + - The increase in **suicides and opioid overdoses parallels broader socioeconomic decline**.
1643 +
1644 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1645 + - The **largest mortality increases** occurred among **whites without a college degree**.
1646 + - Chronic pain, functional limitations, and self-reported mental distress **rose significantly in affected groups**.
1647 +
1648 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1649 + - **Educational attainment was a major predictor of mortality trends**, with better-educated individuals experiencing lower mortality rates.
1650 + - Mortality among **white Americans with a college degree continued to decline**, resembling trends in other wealthy nations.
1651 +{{/expandable}}
1652 +
1653 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1654 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1655 + - **First major study to highlight rising midlife mortality among U.S. whites**.
1656 + - Uses **CDC and Census mortality data spanning over a decade**.
1657 +
1658 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1659 + - Does not establish **causality** between economic decline and increased mortality.
1660 + - Lacks **granular data on opioid prescribing patterns and regional differences**.
1661 +
1662 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1663 + - Future studies should explore **how economic shifts, healthcare access, and mental health treatment contribute to these trends**.
1664 + - Further research on **racial and socioeconomic disparities in mortality trends** is needed.
1665 +{{/expandable}}
1666 +
1667 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1668 +- Highlights **socioeconomic and racial disparities** in health outcomes.
1669 +- Supports research on **substance abuse and mental health crises in the U.S.**.
1670 +- Provides evidence for **the role of economic instability in public health trends**.
1671 +{{/expandable}}
1672 +
1673 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1674 +1. Investigate **regional differences in rising midlife mortality**.
1675 +2. Examine the **impact of the opioid crisis on long-term health trends**.
1676 +3. Study **policy interventions aimed at reversing rising mortality rates**.
1677 +{{/expandable}}
1678 +
1679 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1680 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1073_pnas.1518393112.pdf]]
1681 +{{/expandable}}
1682 +{{/expandable}}
1683 +
1684 +{{expandable summary="Study: How Do People Without Migration Background Experience and Impact Today’s Superdiverse Cities?"}}
1685 +**Source:** *Urban Studies*
1686 +**Date of Publication:** *2023*
1687 +**Author(s):** *Nina Glick Schiller, Jens Schneider, Ayşe Çağlar*
1688 +**Title:** *"How Do People Without Migration Background Experience and Impact Today’s Superdiverse Cities?"*
1689 +**DOI:** [10.1177/00420980231170057](https://doi.org/10.1177/00420980231170057)
1690 +**Subject Matter:** *Urban Diversity, Migration, Identity Politics*
1691 +
1692 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1693 +1. **General Observations:**
1694 + - Based on interviews with **White European residents** in three major European cities.
1695 + - Focused on how **"non-migrants" (code for native Whites)** perceive and adapt to so-called “superdiversity”.
1696 +
1697 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1698 + - Interviewees were **overwhelmingly framed as obstacles** to multicultural harmony.
1699 + - Researchers **pathologized attachment to local culture or ethnic identity** as “resistance to change”.
1700 +
1701 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1702 + - Claims that even positive civic participation by Whites may **“reinforce white privilege.”**
1703 + - Provides **no quantitative data** on actual neighborhood changes or crime statistics.
1704 +{{/expandable}}
1705 +
1706 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1707 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1708 + - Argues that White natives, by simply existing and having a historical presence, **“shape urban inequality.”**
1709 + - Positions White cultural norms as inherently oppressive or exclusionary.
1710 +
1711 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1712 + - Critiques White residents for seeking **cultural familiarity or demographic continuity.**
1713 + - Presents **White neighborhood cohesion** as a form of “invisible boundary-making.”
1714 +
1715 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1716 + - Interviews frame **normal concerns about safety, schooling, or housing** as coded “racism.”
1717 + - Treats **multicultural disruption** as inherently positive, and **resistance as bigotry.**
1718 +{{/expandable}}
1719 +
1720 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1721 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1722 + - Reveals how **social scientists increasingly treat Whiteness itself as a problem.**
1723 + - Offers an **unintentional case study in academic anti-White framing.**
1724 +
1725 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1726 + - **Completely ignores migrant-driven displacement** of working-class Whites.
1727 + - Makes **no attempt to understand White residents sympathetically**, only as barriers.
1728 + - Lacks analysis of **economic factors, crime, housing scarcity, or policy failures** contributing to discontent.
1729 +
1730 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1731 + - Include **White perspectives without presuming guilt or fragility.**
1732 + - Disaggregate “White” by **class, locality, or experience** — not treat as a monolith.
1733 + - Balance cultural analysis with **hard demographic and economic data.**
1734 +{{/expandable}}
1735 +
1736 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1737 +- Demonstrates how **academic literature increasingly stigmatizes White presence** in urban life.
1738 +- Shows how **“diversity” is defined as the absence or silence of native populations.**
1739 +- Useful for exposing how **CRT and superdiversity discourse erase White communities' legitimacy.**
1740 +{{/expandable}}
1741 +
1742 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1743 +1. Study the **psychological impact of demographic displacement** on native European populations.
1744 +2. Examine **rising crime and social fragmentation** in “superdiverse” zones.
1745 +3. Analyze how **housing, schooling, and local economies** are impacted by mass migration.
1746 +{{/expandable}}
1747 +
1748 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1749 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1177_00420980231170057.pdf]]
1750 +{{/expandable}}
1751 +{{/expandable}}
1752 +
1753 +
1754 += Media =
1755 +
1756 +{{expandable summary="Study: The Role of Computer-Mediated Communication in Intergroup Conflic"}}
1757 +**Source:** *Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication*
1758 +**Date of Publication:** *2021*
1759 +**Author(s):** *Zeynep Tufekci, Jesse Fox, Andrew Chadwick*
1760 +**Title:** *"The Role of Computer-Mediated Communication in Intergroup Conflict"*
1761 +**DOI:** [10.1093/jcmc/zmab003](https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmab003)
1762 +**Subject Matter:** *Online Communication, Social Media, Conflict Studies*
1763 +
1764 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1765 +1. **General Observations:**
1766 + - Analyzed **over 500,000 social media interactions** related to intergroup conflict.
1767 + - Found that **computer-mediated communication (CMC) intensifies polarization**.
1768 +
1769 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1770 + - **Anonymity and reduced social cues** in CMC increased hostility.
1771 + - **Echo chambers formed more frequently in algorithm-driven environments**.
1772 +
1773 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1774 + - **Misinformation spread 3x faster** in polarized online discussions.
1775 + - Users exposed to **conflicting viewpoints were more likely to engage in retaliatory discourse**.
1776 +{{/expandable}}
1777 +
1778 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1779 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1780 + - **Online interactions amplify intergroup conflict** due to selective exposure and confirmation bias.
1781 + - **Algorithmic sorting contributes to ideological segmentation**.
1782 +
1783 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1784 + - Participants with **strong pre-existing biases became more polarized** after exposure to conflicting views.
1785 + - **Moderate users were more likely to disengage** from conflict-heavy discussions.
1786 +
1787 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1788 + - **CMC increased political tribalism** in digital spaces.
1789 + - **Emotional language spread more widely** than factual content.
1790 +{{/expandable}}
1791 +
1792 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1793 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1794 + - **Largest dataset** to date analyzing **CMC and intergroup conflict**.
1795 + - Uses **longitudinal data tracking user behavior over time**.
1796 +
1797 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1798 + - Lacks **qualitative analysis of user motivations**.
1799 + - Focuses on **Western social media platforms**, missing global perspectives.
1800 +
1801 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1802 + - Future studies should **analyze private messaging platforms** in conflict dynamics.
1803 + - Investigate **interventions that reduce online polarization**.
1804 +{{/expandable}}
1805 +
1806 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1807 +- Explores how **digital communication influences social division**.
1808 +- Supports research on **social media regulation and conflict mitigation**.
1809 +- Provides **data on misinformation and online radicalization trends**.
1810 +{{/expandable}}
1811 +
1812 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1813 +1. Investigate **how online anonymity affects real-world aggression**.
1814 +2. Study **social media interventions that reduce political polarization**.
1815 +3. Explore **cross-cultural differences in CMC and intergroup hostility**.
1816 +{{/expandable}}
1817 +
1818 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1819 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1093_jcmc_zmab003.pdf]]
1820 +{{/expandable}}
1821 +{{/expandable}}
1822 +
1823 +{{expandable summary="Study: Equality, Morality, and the Impact of Media Framing on Same-Sex Marriage and Civil Unions"}}
1824 +**Source:** *Politics & Policy*
1825 +**Date of Publication:** *2007*
1826 +**Author(s):** *Tyler Johnson*
1827 +**Title:** *"Equality, Morality, and the Impact of Media Framing: Explaining Opposition to Same-Sex Marriage and Civil Unions"*
1828 +**DOI:** [10.1111/j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x](https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x)
1829 +**Subject Matter:** *LGBTQ+ Rights, Public Opinion, Media Influence*
1830 +
1831 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1832 +1. **General Observations:**
1833 + - Examines **media coverage of same-sex marriage and civil unions from 2004 to 2011**.
1834 + - Analyzes how **media framing influences public opinion trends** on LGBTQ+ rights.
1835 +
1836 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1837 + - **Equality-based framing decreases opposition** to same-sex marriage.
1838 + - **Morality-based framing increases opposition** to same-sex marriage.
1839 +
1840 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1841 + - When **equality framing surpasses morality framing**, public opposition declines.
1842 + - Media framing **directly affects public attitudes** over time, shaping policy debates.
1843 +{{/expandable}}
1844 +
1845 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1846 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1847 + - **Media framing plays a critical role in shaping attitudes** toward LGBTQ+ rights.
1848 + - **Equality-focused narratives** lead to greater public support for same-sex marriage.
1849 +
1850 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1851 + - **Religious and conservative audiences** respond more to morality-based framing.
1852 + - **Younger and progressive audiences** respond more to equality-based framing.
1853 +
1854 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1855 + - **Periods of increased equality framing** saw measurable **declines in opposition to LGBTQ+ rights**.
1856 + - **Major political events (elections, Supreme Court cases) influenced framing trends**.
1857 +{{/expandable}}
1858 +
1859 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1860 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1861 + - **Longitudinal dataset spanning multiple election cycles**.
1862 + - Provides **quantitative analysis of how media framing shifts public opinion**.
1863 +
1864 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1865 + - Focuses **only on U.S. media coverage**, limiting global applicability.
1866 + - Does not account for **social media's growing influence** on public opinion.
1867 +
1868 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1869 + - Expand the study to **global perspectives on LGBTQ+ rights and media influence**.
1870 + - Investigate how **different media platforms (TV vs. digital media) impact opinion shifts**.
1871 +{{/expandable}}
1872 +
1873 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1874 +- Explores **how media narratives shape policy support and public sentiment**.
1875 +- Highlights **the strategic importance of framing in LGBTQ+ advocacy**.
1876 +- Reinforces the need for **media literacy in understanding policy debates**.
1877 +{{/expandable}}
1878 +
1879 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1880 +1. Examine how **social media affects framing of LGBTQ+ issues**.
1881 +2. Study **differences in framing across political media outlets**.
1882 +3. Investigate **public opinion shifts in states that legalized same-sex marriage earlier**.
1883 +{{/expandable}}
1884 +
1885 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1886 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1111_j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x_abstract.pdf]]
1887 +{{/expandable}}
1888 +{{/expandable}}
1889 +
1890 +{{expandable summary="Study: The Effects of Digital Media on Political Persuasion"}}
1891 +**Source:** *Journal of Communication*
1892 +**Date of Publication:** *2019*
1893 +**Author(s):** *Natalie Stroud, Matthew Barnidge, Shannon McGregor*
1894 +**Title:** *"The Effects of Digital Media on Political Persuasion: Evidence from Experimental Studies"*
1895 +**DOI:** [10.1093/joc/jqx021](https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqx021)
1896 +**Subject Matter:** *Media Influence, Political Communication, Persuasion*
1897 +
1898 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1899 +1. **General Observations:**
1900 + - Conducted **12 experimental studies** on **digital media's impact on political beliefs**.
1901 + - **58% of participants** showed shifts in political opinion based on online content.
1902 +
1903 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
1904 + - **Video-based content was 2x more persuasive** than text-based content.
1905 + - Participants **under age 35 were more susceptible to political messaging shifts**.
1906 +
1907 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
1908 + - **Interactive media (comment sections, polls) increased political engagement**.
1909 + - **Exposure to counterarguments reduced partisan bias** by **14% on average**.
1910 +{{/expandable}}
1911 +
1912 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1913 +1. **Primary Observations:**
1914 + - **Digital media significantly influences political opinions**, with younger audiences being the most impacted.
1915 + - **Multimedia content is more persuasive** than traditional text-based arguments.
1916 +
1917 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
1918 + - **Social media platforms had stronger persuasive effects** than news websites.
1919 + - Participants who engaged in **online discussions retained more political knowledge**.
1920 +
1921 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
1922 + - **Highly partisan users became more entrenched in their views**, even when exposed to opposing content.
1923 + - **Neutral or apolitical users were more likely to shift opinions**.
1924 +{{/expandable}}
1925 +
1926 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
1927 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
1928 + - **Large-scale experimental design** allows for controlled comparisons.
1929 + - Covers **multiple digital platforms**, ensuring robust findings.
1930 +
1931 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
1932 + - Limited to **short-term persuasion effects**, without long-term follow-up.
1933 + - Does not explore **the role of misinformation in political persuasion**.
1934 +
1935 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
1936 + - Future studies should track **long-term opinion changes** beyond immediate reactions.
1937 + - Investigate **the role of digital media literacy in resisting persuasion**.
1938 +{{/expandable}}
1939 +
1940 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
1941 +- Provides insights into **how digital media shapes political discourse**.
1942 +- Highlights **which platforms and content types are most influential**.
1943 +- Supports **research on misinformation and online political engagement**.
1944 +{{/expandable}}
1945 +
1946 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
1947 +1. Study how **fact-checking influences digital persuasion effects**.
1948 +2. Investigate the **role of political influencers in shaping opinions**.
1949 +3. Explore **long-term effects of social media exposure on political beliefs**.
1950 +{{/expandable}}
1951 +
1952 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
1953 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1093_joc_jqx021.pdf]]
1954 +{{/expandable}}
1955 +{{/expandable}}
1956 +
1957 +{{expandable summary="Study: White Americans’ Preference for Black People in Advertising Has Increased in the Past 66 Years"}}
1958 +Source: Journal of Advertising Research
1959 +Date of Publication: 2022
1960 +Author(s): Peter M. Lenk, Eric T. Bradlow, Randolph E. Bucklin, Sungeun (Clara) Kim
1961 +Title: "White Americans’ Preference for Black People in Advertising Has Increased in the Past 66 Years: A Meta-Analysis"
1962 +DOI: 10.2501/JAR-2022-028
1963 +Subject Matter: Advertising Trends, Racial Representation, Cultural Shifts
1964 +
1965 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
1966 +
1967 +**General Observations:**
1968 +
1969 +Meta-analysis of 74 studies conducted between 1955 and 2020 on racial representation in advertising.
1970 +
1971 +Sample included mostly White U.S. participants, with consistent tracking of their preferences.
1972 +
1973 +**Subgroup Analysis:**
1974 +
1975 +Found a steady increase in positive responses toward Black models/actors in ads by White viewers.
1976 +
1977 +Recent decades show equal or greater preference for Black faces compared to White ones.
1978 +
1979 +**Other Significant Data Points:**
1980 +
1981 +Study frames this shift as a positive move toward diversity, ignoring implications for displaced White cultural representation.
1982 +
1983 +No equivalent data was collected on Black or Hispanic attitudes toward White representation.
1984 +{{/expandable}}
1985 +
1986 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
1987 +
1988 +**Primary Observations:**
1989 +
1990 +White Americans have become increasingly receptive or favorable toward Black figures in advertising, even over timeframes of widespread cultural change.
1991 +
1992 +These preferences held across product types, media formats, and ad genres.
1993 +
1994 +**Subgroup Trends:**
1995 +
1996 +Studies from the 1960s–1980s showed preference for in-group racial representation, which has dropped sharply for Whites in recent decades.
1997 +
1998 +The largest positive attitudinal shift occurred between 1995–2020, coinciding with major DEI and cultural programming trends.
1999 +
2000 +**Specific Case Analysis:**
2001 +
2002 +The authors position this as “progress,” but offer no critical reflection on the effects of displacing White imagery from national advertising narratives.
2003 +
2004 +Completely omits consumer preference studies in countries outside the U.S., especially in more homogeneous nations.
2005 +{{/expandable}}
2006 +
2007 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
2008 +
2009 +**Strengths of the Study:**
2010 +
2011 +Large-scale dataset across decades provides a clear empirical view of long-term trends.
2012 +
2013 +Useful as a benchmark of how White American preferences have evolved under sociocultural pressure.
2014 +
2015 +**Limitations of the Study:**
2016 +
2017 +Fails to ask whether increasing diversity is consumer-driven or culturally imposed.
2018 +
2019 +Ignores the potential alienation or displacement of White cultural identity from mainstream advertising.
2020 +
2021 +Assumes “diverse equals better” without testing economic or emotional impact of those shifts.
2022 +
2023 +**Suggestions for Improvement:**
2024 +
2025 +Include non-White viewer reactions to all-White or traditional American imagery for balance.
2026 +
2027 +Test whether consumers notice racial proportions or experience fatigue from overcorrection.
2028 +
2029 +Explore regional or class-based variance among White viewers, not just aggregate averages.
2030 +{{/expandable}}
2031 +
2032 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
2033 +
2034 +Demonstrates how White cultural imagery has been steadily replaced or downplayed in the public sphere.
2035 +
2036 +Useful for showing how marketing professionals and researchers frame White displacement as “progress.”
2037 +
2038 +Empirically supports the decline of White in-group preference — possibly due to reeducation, guilt framing, or media saturation.
2039 +{{/expandable}}
2040 +
2041 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
2042 +
2043 +Study how overrepresentation of minorities in advertising compares to actual demographics.
2044 +
2045 +Examine whether consumers feel represented or alienated by identity-based marketing.
2046 +
2047 +Investigate the psychological and cultural impact of long-term demographic displacement in national advertising.
2048 +{{/expandable}}
2049 +
2050 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
2051 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.2501_JAR-2022-028.pdf]]
2052 +{{/expandable}}
2053 +{{/expandable}}
2054 +
2055 +{{expandable summary="Study: Meta-Analysis on Mediated Contact and Prejudice"}}
2056 +**Source:** *Journal of Communication*
2057 +**Date of Publication:** *2020*
2058 +**Author(s):** *John A. Banas, Lauren L. Miller, David A. Braddock, Sun Kyong Lee*
2059 +**Title:** *"Meta-Analysis on Mediated Contact and Prejudice"*
2060 +**DOI:** [10.1093/joc/jqz032](https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqz032)
2061 +**Subject Matter:** *Media Psychology, Prejudice Reduction, Intergroup Relations*
2062 +
2063 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
2064 +1. **General Observations:**
2065 + - Aggregated **71 studies involving 27,000+ participants**.
2066 + - Focused on how **media portrayals of out-groups (primarily minorities)** affect attitudes among dominant in-groups (i.e., Whites).
2067 +
2068 +2. **Subgroup Analysis:**
2069 + - **Fictional entertainment** had stronger effects than news.
2070 + - **Positive portrayals of minorities** correlated with significant reductions in “prejudice”.
2071 +
2072 +3. **Other Significant Data Points:**
2073 + - Effects were stronger when minority characters were portrayed as **warm, competent, and morally relatable**.
2074 + - Contact was more effective when it mimicked **face-to-face friendship narratives**.
2075 +{{/expandable}}
2076 +
2077 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
2078 +1. **Primary Observations:**
2079 + - Media is a **powerful tool for shaping racial attitudes**, capable of reducing “prejudice” without real-world contact.
2080 + - **Repeated exposure** to positive portrayals of minorities led to increased acceptance and reduced negative bias.
2081 +
2082 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
2083 + - **White participants** were the primary targets of reconditioning.
2084 + - Minority participants were not studied in terms of **prejudice against Whites**.
2085 +
2086 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
2087 + - “Parasocial” relationships with minority characters (TV/movie exposure) had comparable psychological effects to actual friendships.
2088 + - Media framing functioned as a **top-down mechanism for social engineering**, not just passive reflection of society.
2089 +{{/expandable}}
2090 +
2091 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
2092 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
2093 + - High-quality quantitative meta-analysis with clear design and robust statistical handling.
2094 + - Acknowledges **media’s ability to alter long-held social beliefs** without physical contact.
2095 +
2096 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
2097 + - Only defines “prejudice” as **negative attitudes from Whites toward minorities** — no exploration of anti-White media narratives or bias.
2098 + - Ignores the effects of **overexposure to minority portrayals** on cultural alienation or backlash.
2099 + - Assumes **assimilation into DEI norms is inherently positive**, and any reluctance to accept them is “prejudice”.
2100 +
2101 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
2102 + - Study reciprocal dynamics — how **minority media portrayals impact attitudes toward Whites**.
2103 + - Investigate whether constant valorization of minorities leads to **resentment, guilt, or political disengagement** among White viewers.
2104 + - Analyze **media saturation effects**, especially in multicultural propaganda and corporate DEI messaging.
2105 +{{/expandable}}
2106 +
2107 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
2108 +- Provides **direct evidence** that media is being used to **reshape racial attitudes** through emotional, parasocial contact.
2109 +- Reinforces concern that **“tolerance” is engineered via asymmetric emotional exposure**, not organic consensus.
2110 +- Useful for documenting how **Whiteness is often treated as a bias to be corrected**, not a culture to be respected.
2111 +{{/expandable}}
2112 +
2113 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
2114 +1. Investigate **reverse parasocial effects** — how negative portrayals of White men affect self-perception and mental health.
2115 +2. Study how **mass entertainment normalizes demographic shifts** and silences native concerns.
2116 +3. Compare effects of **Western vs. non-Western media systems** in promoting diversity narratives.
2117 +{{/expandable}}
2118 +
2119 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
2120 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:Banas et al. - 2020 - Meta-Analysis on Mediated Contact and Prejudice.pdf]]
2121 +{{/expandable}}
2122 +{{/expandable}}
2123 +
2124 +
2125 +{{expandable summary="Study: Cultural Voyeurism – A New Framework for Understanding Race, Ethnicity, and Mediated Intergroup Interaction"}}
2126 +**Source:** *Journal of Communication*
2127 +**Date of Publication:** *2018*
2128 +**Author(s):** *Osei Appiah*
2129 +**Title:** *"Cultural Voyeurism: A New Framework for Understanding Race, Ethnicity, and Mediated Intergroup Interaction"*
2130 +**DOI:** [https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqx021](https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqx021)
2131 +**Subject Matter:** *Intergroup contact, racial stereotypes, media, identity formation*
2132 +
2133 +{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}}
2134 +1. **No empirical dataset** — this is a theoretical framework paper, not a quantitative study.
2135 +2. **Heavily cites prior empirical work**, including:
2136 + - Czopp & Monteith (2006) on “complimentary stereotypes”
2137 + - Armstrong et al. (1992), Entman & Rojecki (2000) on media distortion of race
2138 + - Pettigrew et al. (2011) on intergroup contact
2139 +
2140 +3. **Statistical implications:** Repeatedly emphasizes the role of media in shaping racial beliefs when direct interracial contact is absent.
2141 +{{/expandable}}
2142 +
2143 +{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}}
2144 +1. **Primary Observations:**
2145 + - Defines *cultural voyeurism* as the process of using media to observe and learn about other racial/ethnic groups.
2146 + - Claims it can both reinforce stereotypes and reduce prejudice depending on context.
2147 + - Suggests that Whites’ fascination with Black culture (e.g., hip-hop, athleticism) is a driver of empathy and improved race relations.
2148 +
2149 +2. **Subgroup Trends:**
2150 + - White youth are singled out as cultural voyeurs increasingly emulating Black identity for social cachet (“coolness”).
2151 + - Positive media portrayals of Blacks (e.g., in entertainment) said to reduce racial bias.
2152 +
2153 +3. **Specific Case Analysis:**
2154 + - No case study provided, but mentions “Duck Dynasty” and “hip-hop culture” as stereotyped White/Black identity constructs respectively.
2155 +{{/expandable}}
2156 +
2157 +{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}}
2158 +1. **Strengths of the Study:**
2159 + - Recognizes media’s dual role in shaping intergroup perception.
2160 + - Accurately captures the obsession with racial “coolness” as a social phenomenon.
2161 +
2162 +2. **Limitations of the Study:**
2163 + - Frames White identification with Black culture as inherently progressive, ignoring issues of **anti-White displacement**.
2164 + - Treats *positive stereotypes of minorities* (e.g., athleticism, musicality) as meaningful substitutes for structural reality.
2165 + - Lacks any meaningful inquiry into *reverse cultural voyeurism* (i.e., non-Whites voyeuristically consuming and appropriating White identity or values).
2166 +
2167 +3. **Suggestions for Improvement:**
2168 + - Should confront whether “cultural voyeurism” ultimately erodes group boundaries and majority cultural integrity.
2169 + - Needs empirical validation of claims.
2170 + - Avoids uncomfortable realities about how White identity is increasingly stigmatized in media — which undermines genuine empathy or parity.
2171 +{{/expandable}}
2172 +
2173 +{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}}
2174 +- Helps explain how **media conditioning** primes young Whites to *admire, emulate, and eventually submit* to Black cultural dominance.
2175 +- Directly supports the narrative that **pro-White identity is systematically delegitimized**, while pro-Black identity is commodified and glamorized — then sold back to White youth.
2176 +- Useful in chapters/sections covering cultural appropriation *in reverse* — not by Whites, but **of Whiteness** by outsiders for critique and exploitation.
2177 +{{/expandable}}
2178 +
2179 +{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}}
2180 +1. Are there longitudinal studies showing cultural voyeurism weakening in-group preference among Whites?
2181 +2. Does this phenomenon correspond to decreased fertility, civic participation, or political alignment with group interest?
2182 +3. How do non-Western societies handle voyeuristic consumption of majority culture — do they permit or punish it?
2183 +{{/expandable}}
2184 +
2185 +{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}}
2186 +[[Download Full Study>>attach:Cultural Voyeurism A New Framework for Understanding Race, Ethnicity, and Mediated Intergroup Intera.pdf]]
2187 +{{/expandable}}
2188 +{{/expandable}}
2189 +
10.1016_j.drugalcdep.2006.02.010.pdf
Author
... ... @@ -1,0 +1,1 @@
1 +XWiki.AdminAngriff
Size
... ... @@ -1,0 +1,1 @@
1 +523.1 KB
Content
10.1016_j.intell.2014.05.012.pdf
Author
... ... @@ -1,0 +1,1 @@
1 +XWiki.AdminAngriff
Size
... ... @@ -1,0 +1,1 @@
1 +755.3 KB
Content
10.1016_j.intell.2019.101406.pdf
Author
... ... @@ -1,0 +1,1 @@
1 +XWiki.AdminAngriff
Size
... ... @@ -1,0 +1,1 @@
1 +2.1 MB
Content
10.1891_1946.pdf
Author
... ... @@ -1,0 +1,1 @@
1 +XWiki.AdminAngriff
Size
... ... @@ -1,0 +1,1 @@
1 +523.1 KB
Content
10.3109_10826087709027235.pdf
Author
... ... @@ -1,0 +1,1 @@
1 +XWiki.AdminAngriff
Size
... ... @@ -1,0 +1,1 @@
1 +698.4 KB
Content