... |
... |
@@ -1122,6 +1122,76 @@ |
1122 |
1122 |
|
1123 |
1123 |
= Whiteness & White Guilt = |
1124 |
1124 |
|
|
1125 |
+{{expandable summary="Study: Reducing Implicit Racial Preferences: I. A Comparative Investigation of 17 Interventions"}} |
|
1126 |
+**Source:** *Psychological Science* |
|
1127 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2014* |
|
1128 |
+**Author(s):** *Caleb E. Lai, Anthony G. Greenwald, et al.* |
|
1129 |
+**Title:** *"Reducing Implicit Racial Preferences: I. A Comparative Investigation of 17 Interventions"* |
|
1130 |
+**DOI:** [10.1177/0956797614535812](https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614535812) |
|
1131 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Implicit Bias, Racial Psychology, Psychological Conditioning* |
|
1132 |
+ |
|
1133 |
+{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
|
1134 |
+1. **General Observations:** |
|
1135 |
+ - Tested **17 different interventions** across **6,321 participants**, all measured via IAT (Implicit Association Test). |
|
1136 |
+ - Focused exclusively on reducing **pro-White, anti-Black preferences** — no reciprocal testing on anti-White bias. |
|
1137 |
+ |
|
1138 |
+2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
|
1139 |
+ - Educational and exposure-based interventions (e.g., multiculturalism, egalitarian messaging) failed to reduce bias significantly. |
|
1140 |
+ - Most effective short-term results came from **trauma-based or emotionally coercive interventions**. |
|
1141 |
+ |
|
1142 |
+3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
|
1143 |
+ - The **"Black hero" intervention**, where participants imagined being violently attacked by a White man and rescued by a Black man, was among the most effective. |
|
1144 |
+ - Effects of even the most extreme interventions **dissipated within 24–72 hours**, with no long-term behavioral change. |
|
1145 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1146 |
+ |
|
1147 |
+{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
|
1148 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
1149 |
+ - The interventions that produced the most dramatic IAT changes used **emotionally graphic narratives** depicting Whites as violent aggressors and Blacks as saviors. |
|
1150 |
+ - Merely showing positive Black images or promoting egalitarian values had minimal effect on implicit associations. |
|
1151 |
+ |
|
1152 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
1153 |
+ - In the **"Black hero" condition**, participants were asked to imagine being physically beaten by a White person and then rescued by a Black person — an intentionally vivid and disturbing scenario. |
|
1154 |
+ - The **"Black victim" intervention** relied on emotionally shocking imagery of anti-Black violence (e.g., lynching) to induce guilt and disrupt positive associations with Whiteness. |
|
1155 |
+ |
|
1156 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
1157 |
+ - None of the scenarios reversed the framing (e.g., Black aggressor/White victim), confirming the ideological goal was **to degrade White identity**, not merely reduce bias. |
|
1158 |
+ - The study was **cited by the White House Office of Science and Technology Policy (OSTP)** to justify DEI-aligned policy recommendations. |
|
1159 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1160 |
+ |
|
1161 |
+{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
|
1162 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
1163 |
+ - Large sample size and systematic comparison across diverse intervention types. |
|
1164 |
+ - Clearly shows that **implicit preference is resilient** and not easily changed by education or exposure alone. |
|
1165 |
+ |
|
1166 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
1167 |
+ - The most “effective” methods **relied on emotional manipulation, not persuasion or evidence**. |
|
1168 |
+ - Assumes **natural in-group preference is pathological** when expressed by White subjects but makes no effort to test other groups. |
|
1169 |
+ - **Zero attention to pro-Black or anti-White bias** — only White attitudes are pathologized. |
|
1170 |
+ |
|
1171 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
1172 |
+ - Test the **psychological harm** and ethical implications of using graphic racial trauma to coerce attitude change. |
|
1173 |
+ - Include interventions that **strengthen ingroup empathy** without demonizing other groups. |
|
1174 |
+ - Disaggregate bias by **class, region, and individual experience**, rather than racially reducing all bias to “Whiteness.” |
|
1175 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1176 |
+ |
|
1177 |
+{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
|
1178 |
+- Provides direct evidence that **DEI-style implicit bias training** is based on emotionally abusive and **anti-White psychological framing**. |
|
1179 |
+- Shows how **social science selectively targets Whites for attitude correction**, often using fictionalized racial trauma scenarios. |
|
1180 |
+- Demonstrates that even extreme interventions **fail to achieve long-term change**, undermining the scientific justification for such policies. |
|
1181 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1182 |
+ |
|
1183 |
+{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
|
1184 |
+1. Investigate **implicit bias training outcomes** in real-world institutional settings. |
|
1185 |
+2. Study **the ethical limits of psychological reprogramming** in DEI policies. |
|
1186 |
+3. Explore **natural ingroup preference across all races** using morally neutral frameworks. |
|
1187 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1188 |
+ |
|
1189 |
+{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
|
1190 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:lai2014.pdf]] |
|
1191 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1192 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1193 |
+ |
|
1194 |
+ |
1125 |
1125 |
{{expandable summary="Study: Segregation, Innocence, and Protection: The Institutional Conditions That Maintain Whiteness in College Sports"}} |
1126 |
1126 |
**Source:** *Journal of Diversity in Higher Education* |
1127 |
1127 |
**Date of Publication:** *2019* |
... |
... |
@@ -1770,3 +1770,69 @@ |
1770 |
1770 |
{{/expandable}} |
1771 |
1771 |
{{/expandable}} |
1772 |
1772 |
|
|
1843 |
+ |
|
1844 |
+{{expandable summary="Study: Cultural Voyeurism – A New Framework for Understanding Race, Ethnicity, and Mediated Intergroup Interaction"}} |
|
1845 |
+**Source:** *Journal of Communication* |
|
1846 |
+**Date of Publication:** *2018* |
|
1847 |
+**Author(s):** *Osei Appiah* |
|
1848 |
+**Title:** *"Cultural Voyeurism: A New Framework for Understanding Race, Ethnicity, and Mediated Intergroup Interaction"* |
|
1849 |
+**DOI:** [https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqx021](https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqx021) |
|
1850 |
+**Subject Matter:** *Intergroup contact, racial stereotypes, media, identity formation* |
|
1851 |
+ |
|
1852 |
+{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
|
1853 |
+1. **No empirical dataset** — this is a theoretical framework paper, not a quantitative study. |
|
1854 |
+2. **Heavily cites prior empirical work**, including: |
|
1855 |
+ - Czopp & Monteith (2006) on “complimentary stereotypes” |
|
1856 |
+ - Armstrong et al. (1992), Entman & Rojecki (2000) on media distortion of race |
|
1857 |
+ - Pettigrew et al. (2011) on intergroup contact |
|
1858 |
+ |
|
1859 |
+3. **Statistical implications:** Repeatedly emphasizes the role of media in shaping racial beliefs when direct interracial contact is absent. |
|
1860 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1861 |
+ |
|
1862 |
+{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
|
1863 |
+1. **Primary Observations:** |
|
1864 |
+ - Defines *cultural voyeurism* as the process of using media to observe and learn about other racial/ethnic groups. |
|
1865 |
+ - Claims it can both reinforce stereotypes and reduce prejudice depending on context. |
|
1866 |
+ - Suggests that Whites’ fascination with Black culture (e.g., hip-hop, athleticism) is a driver of empathy and improved race relations. |
|
1867 |
+ |
|
1868 |
+2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
|
1869 |
+ - White youth are singled out as cultural voyeurs increasingly emulating Black identity for social cachet (“coolness”). |
|
1870 |
+ - Positive media portrayals of Blacks (e.g., in entertainment) said to reduce racial bias. |
|
1871 |
+ |
|
1872 |
+3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
|
1873 |
+ - No case study provided, but mentions “Duck Dynasty” and “hip-hop culture” as stereotyped White/Black identity constructs respectively. |
|
1874 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1875 |
+ |
|
1876 |
+{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
|
1877 |
+1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
|
1878 |
+ - Recognizes media’s dual role in shaping intergroup perception. |
|
1879 |
+ - Accurately captures the obsession with racial “coolness” as a social phenomenon. |
|
1880 |
+ |
|
1881 |
+2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
|
1882 |
+ - Frames White identification with Black culture as inherently progressive, ignoring issues of **anti-White displacement**. |
|
1883 |
+ - Treats *positive stereotypes of minorities* (e.g., athleticism, musicality) as meaningful substitutes for structural reality. |
|
1884 |
+ - Lacks any meaningful inquiry into *reverse cultural voyeurism* (i.e., non-Whites voyeuristically consuming and appropriating White identity or values). |
|
1885 |
+ |
|
1886 |
+3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
|
1887 |
+ - Should confront whether “cultural voyeurism” ultimately erodes group boundaries and majority cultural integrity. |
|
1888 |
+ - Needs empirical validation of claims. |
|
1889 |
+ - Avoids uncomfortable realities about how White identity is increasingly stigmatized in media — which undermines genuine empathy or parity. |
|
1890 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1891 |
+ |
|
1892 |
+{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
|
1893 |
+- Helps explain how **media conditioning** primes young Whites to *admire, emulate, and eventually submit* to Black cultural dominance. |
|
1894 |
+- Directly supports the narrative that **pro-White identity is systematically delegitimized**, while pro-Black identity is commodified and glamorized — then sold back to White youth. |
|
1895 |
+- Useful in chapters/sections covering cultural appropriation *in reverse* — not by Whites, but **of Whiteness** by outsiders for critique and exploitation. |
|
1896 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1897 |
+ |
|
1898 |
+{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
|
1899 |
+1. Are there longitudinal studies showing cultural voyeurism weakening in-group preference among Whites? |
|
1900 |
+2. Does this phenomenon correspond to decreased fertility, civic participation, or political alignment with group interest? |
|
1901 |
+3. How do non-Western societies handle voyeuristic consumption of majority culture — do they permit or punish it? |
|
1902 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1903 |
+ |
|
1904 |
+{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
|
1905 |
+[[Download Full Study>>attach:Cultural Voyeurism A New Framework for Understanding Race, Ethnicity, and Mediated Intergroup Intera.pdf]] |
|
1906 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1907 |
+{{/expandable}} |
|
1908 |
+ |