... |
... |
@@ -19,1576 +19,7 @@ |
19 |
19 |
- You'll also find a download link to the original full study in pdf form at the bottom of the collapsible block. |
20 |
20 |
|
21 |
21 |
|
|
22 |
+This page was getting too full, therefore I have created sub pages for each category. This makes it much easier to add new studies. |
22 |
22 |
|
23 |
|
-= Genetics = |
24 |
24 |
|
25 |
|
-{{expandable summary=" |
26 |
|
- |
27 |
|
-Study: Reconstructing Indian Population History"}} |
28 |
|
-**Source:** *Nature* |
29 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2009* |
30 |
|
-**Author(s):** *David Reich, Kumarasamy Thangaraj, Nick Patterson, Alkes L. Price, Lalji Singh* |
31 |
|
-**Title:** *"Reconstructing Indian Population History"* |
32 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1038/nature08365](https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08365) |
33 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Genetics, Population History, South Asian Ancestry* |
34 |
|
- |
35 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
36 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
37 |
|
- - Study analyzed **132 individuals from 25 diverse Indian groups**. |
38 |
|
- - Identified two major ancestral populations: **Ancestral North Indians (ANI)** and **Ancestral South Indians (ASI)**. |
39 |
|
- |
40 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
41 |
|
- - ANI ancestry is closely related to **Middle Easterners, Central Asians, and Europeans**. |
42 |
|
- - ASI ancestry is **genetically distinct from ANI and East Asians**. |
43 |
|
- |
44 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
45 |
|
- - ANI ancestry ranges from **39% to 71%** across Indian groups. |
46 |
|
- - **Caste and linguistic differences** strongly correlate with genetic variation. |
47 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
48 |
|
- |
49 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
50 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
51 |
|
- - The genetic landscape of India has been shaped by **thousands of years of endogamy**. |
52 |
|
- - Groups with **only ASI ancestry no longer exist** in mainland India. |
53 |
|
- |
54 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
55 |
|
- - **Higher ANI ancestry in upper-caste and Indo-European-speaking groups**. |
56 |
|
- - **Andaman Islanders** are unique in having **ASI ancestry without ANI influence**. |
57 |
|
- |
58 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
59 |
|
- - **Founder effects** have maintained allele frequency differences among Indian groups. |
60 |
|
- - Predicts **higher incidence of recessive diseases** due to historical genetic isolation. |
61 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
62 |
|
- |
63 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
64 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
65 |
|
- - **First large-scale genetic analysis** of Indian population history. |
66 |
|
- - Introduces **new methods for ancestry estimation without direct ancestral reference groups**. |
67 |
|
- |
68 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
69 |
|
- - Limited **sample size relative to India's population diversity**. |
70 |
|
- - Does not include **recent admixture events** post-colonial era. |
71 |
|
- |
72 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
73 |
|
- - Future research should **expand sampling across more Indian tribal groups**. |
74 |
|
- - Use **whole-genome sequencing** for finer resolution of ancestry. |
75 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
76 |
|
- |
77 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
78 |
|
-- Provides a **genetic basis for caste and linguistic diversity** in India. |
79 |
|
-- Highlights **founder effects and genetic drift** shaping South Asian populations. |
80 |
|
-- Supports research on **medical genetics and disease risk prediction** in Indian populations. |
81 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
82 |
|
- |
83 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
84 |
|
-1. Examine **genetic markers linked to disease susceptibility** in Indian subpopulations. |
85 |
|
-2. Investigate the impact of **recent migration patterns on ANI-ASI ancestry distribution**. |
86 |
|
-3. Study **gene flow between Indian populations and other global groups**. |
87 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
88 |
|
- |
89 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
90 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nature08365.pdf]] |
91 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
92 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
93 |
|
- |
94 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: The Simons Genome Diversity Project: 300 Genomes from 142 Diverse Populations"}} |
95 |
|
-**Source:** *Nature* |
96 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2016* |
97 |
|
-**Author(s):** *David Reich, Swapan Mallick, Heng Li, Mark Lipson, and others* |
98 |
|
-**Title:** *"The Simons Genome Diversity Project: 300 Genomes from 142 Diverse Populations"* |
99 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1038/nature18964](https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18964) |
100 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Human Genetic Diversity, Population History, Evolutionary Genomics* |
101 |
|
- |
102 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
103 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
104 |
|
- - Analyzed **high-coverage genome sequences of 300 individuals from 142 populations**. |
105 |
|
- - Included **many underrepresented and indigenous groups** from Africa, Asia, Europe, and the Americas. |
106 |
|
- |
107 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
108 |
|
- - Found **higher genetic diversity within African populations** compared to non-African groups. |
109 |
|
- - Showed **Neanderthal and Denisovan ancestry in non-African populations**, particularly in Oceania. |
110 |
|
- |
111 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
112 |
|
- - Identified **5.8 million base pairs absent from the human reference genome**. |
113 |
|
- - Estimated that **mutations have accumulated 5% faster in non-Africans than in Africans**. |
114 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
115 |
|
- |
116 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
117 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
118 |
|
- - **African populations harbor the greatest genetic diversity**, confirming an out-of-Africa dispersal model. |
119 |
|
- - Indigenous Australians and New Guineans **share a common ancestral population with other non-Africans**. |
120 |
|
- |
121 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
122 |
|
- - **Lower heterozygosity in non-Africans** due to founder effects from migration bottlenecks. |
123 |
|
- - **Denisovan ancestry in South Asians is higher than previously thought**. |
124 |
|
- |
125 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
126 |
|
- - **Neanderthal ancestry is higher in East Asians than in Europeans**. |
127 |
|
- - African hunter-gatherer groups show **deep population splits over 100,000 years ago**. |
128 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
129 |
|
- |
130 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
131 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
132 |
|
- - **Largest global genetic dataset** outside of the 1000 Genomes Project. |
133 |
|
- - High sequencing depth allows **more accurate identification of genetic variants**. |
134 |
|
- |
135 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
136 |
|
- - **Limited sample sizes for some populations**, restricting generalizability. |
137 |
|
- - Lacks ancient DNA comparisons, making it difficult to reconstruct deep ancestry fully. |
138 |
|
- |
139 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
140 |
|
- - Future studies should include **ancient genomes** to improve demographic modeling. |
141 |
|
- - Expand research into **how genetic variation affects health outcomes** across populations. |
142 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
143 |
|
- |
144 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
145 |
|
-- Provides **comprehensive data on human genetic diversity**, useful for **evolutionary studies**. |
146 |
|
-- Supports research on **Neanderthal and Denisovan introgression** in modern human populations. |
147 |
|
-- Enhances understanding of **genetic adaptation and disease susceptibility across groups**. |
148 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
149 |
|
- |
150 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
151 |
|
-1. Investigate **functional consequences of genetic variation in underrepresented populations**. |
152 |
|
-2. Study **how selection pressures shaped genetic diversity across different environments**. |
153 |
|
-3. Explore **medical applications of population-specific genetic markers**. |
154 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
155 |
|
- |
156 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
157 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nature18964.pdf]] |
158 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
159 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
160 |
|
- |
161 |
|
-{{expandable summary=" |
162 |
|
- |
163 |
|
-Study: Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies"}} |
164 |
|
-**Source:** *Nature Genetics* |
165 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2015* |
166 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Tinca J. C. Polderman, Beben Benyamin, Christiaan A. de Leeuw, Patrick F. Sullivan, Arjen van Bochoven, Peter M. Visscher, Danielle Posthuma* |
167 |
|
-**Title:** *"Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies"* |
168 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1038/ng.328](https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.328) |
169 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Genetics, Heritability, Twin Studies, Behavioral Science* |
170 |
|
- |
171 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
172 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
173 |
|
- - Analyzed **17,804 traits from 2,748 twin studies** published between **1958 and 2012**. |
174 |
|
- - Included data from **14,558,903 twin pairs**, making it the largest meta-analysis on human heritability. |
175 |
|
- |
176 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
177 |
|
- - Found **49% average heritability** across all traits. |
178 |
|
- - **69% of traits follow a simple additive genetic model**, meaning most variance is due to genes, not environment. |
179 |
|
- |
180 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
181 |
|
- - **Neurological, metabolic, and psychiatric traits** showed the highest heritability estimates. |
182 |
|
- - Traits related to **social values and environmental interactions** had lower heritability estimates. |
183 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
184 |
|
- |
185 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
186 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
187 |
|
- - Across all traits, genetic factors play a significant role in individual differences. |
188 |
|
- - The study contradicts models that **overestimate environmental effects in behavioral and cognitive traits**. |
189 |
|
- |
190 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
191 |
|
- - **Eye and brain-related traits showed the highest heritability (70-80%)**. |
192 |
|
- - **Shared environmental effects were negligible (<10%) for most traits**. |
193 |
|
- |
194 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
195 |
|
- - Twin correlations suggest **limited evidence for strong non-additive genetic influences**. |
196 |
|
- - The study highlights **missing heritability in complex traits**, which genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have yet to fully explain. |
197 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
198 |
|
- |
199 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
200 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
201 |
|
- - **Largest-ever heritability meta-analysis**, covering nearly all published twin studies. |
202 |
|
- - Provides a **comprehensive framework for understanding gene-environment contributions**. |
203 |
|
- |
204 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
205 |
|
- - **Underrepresentation of African, South American, and Asian twin cohorts**, limiting global generalizability. |
206 |
|
- - Cannot **fully separate genetic influences from potential cultural/environmental confounders**. |
207 |
|
- |
208 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
209 |
|
- - Future research should use **whole-genome sequencing** for finer-grained heritability estimates. |
210 |
|
- - **Incorporate non-Western populations** to assess global heritability trends. |
211 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
212 |
|
- |
213 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
214 |
|
-- Establishes a **quantitative benchmark for heritability across human traits**. |
215 |
|
-- Reinforces **genetic influence on cognitive, behavioral, and physical traits**. |
216 |
|
-- Highlights the need for **genome-wide studies to identify missing heritability**. |
217 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
218 |
|
- |
219 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
220 |
|
-1. Investigate how **heritability estimates compare across different socioeconomic backgrounds**. |
221 |
|
-2. Examine **gene-environment interactions in cognitive and psychiatric traits**. |
222 |
|
-3. Explore **non-additive genetic effects on human traits using newer statistical models**. |
223 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
224 |
|
- |
225 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
226 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_ng.328.pdf]] |
227 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
228 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
229 |
|
- |
230 |
|
-{{expandable summary=" |
231 |
|
- |
232 |
|
-Study: Genetic Analysis of African Populations: Human Evolution and Complex Disease"}} |
233 |
|
-**Source:** *Nature Reviews Genetics* |
234 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2002* |
235 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Sarah A. Tishkoff, Scott M. Williams* |
236 |
|
-**Title:** *"Genetic Analysis of African Populations: Human Evolution and Complex Disease"* |
237 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1038/nrg865](https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg865) |
238 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Population Genetics, Human Evolution, Complex Diseases* |
239 |
|
- |
240 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
241 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
242 |
|
- - Africa harbors **the highest genetic diversity** of any region, making it key to understanding human evolution. |
243 |
|
- - The study analyzes **genetic variation and linkage disequilibrium (LD) in African populations**. |
244 |
|
- |
245 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
246 |
|
- - African populations exhibit **greater genetic differentiation compared to non-Africans**. |
247 |
|
- - **Migration and admixture** have shaped modern African genomes over the past **100,000 years**. |
248 |
|
- |
249 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
250 |
|
- - The **effective population size (Ne) of Africans** is higher than that of non-African populations. |
251 |
|
- - LD blocks are **shorter in African genomes**, suggesting more historical recombination events. |
252 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
253 |
|
- |
254 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
255 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
256 |
|
- - African populations are the **most genetically diverse**, supporting the *Recent African Origin* hypothesis. |
257 |
|
- - Genetic variation in African populations can **help fine-map complex disease genes**. |
258 |
|
- |
259 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
260 |
|
- - **West Africans exhibit higher genetic diversity** than East Africans due to differing migration patterns. |
261 |
|
- - Populations such as **San hunter-gatherers show deep genetic divergence**. |
262 |
|
- |
263 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
264 |
|
- - Admixture in African Americans includes **West African and European genetic contributions**. |
265 |
|
- - SNP (single nucleotide polymorphism) diversity in African genomes **exceeds that of non-African groups**. |
266 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
267 |
|
- |
268 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
269 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
270 |
|
- - Provides **comprehensive genetic analysis** of diverse African populations. |
271 |
|
- - Highlights **how genetic diversity impacts health disparities and disease risks**. |
272 |
|
- |
273 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
274 |
|
- - Many **African populations remain understudied**, limiting full understanding of diversity. |
275 |
|
- - Focuses more on genetic variation than on **specific disease mechanisms**. |
276 |
|
- |
277 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
278 |
|
- - Expand research into **underrepresented African populations**. |
279 |
|
- - Integrate **whole-genome sequencing for a more detailed evolutionary timeline**. |
280 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
281 |
|
- |
282 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
283 |
|
-- Supports **genetic models of human evolution** and the **out-of-Africa hypothesis**. |
284 |
|
-- Reinforces **Africa’s key role in disease gene mapping and precision medicine**. |
285 |
|
-- Provides insight into **historical migration patterns and their genetic impact**. |
286 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
287 |
|
- |
288 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
289 |
|
-1. Investigate **genetic adaptations to local environments within Africa**. |
290 |
|
-2. Study **the role of African genetic diversity in disease resistance**. |
291 |
|
-3. Expand research on **how ancient migration patterns shaped modern genetic structure**. |
292 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
293 |
|
- |
294 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
295 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1038_nrg865MODERN.pdf]] |
296 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
297 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
298 |
|
- |
299 |
|
-{{expandable summary=" |
300 |
|
- |
301 |
|
-Study: Pervasive Findings of Directional Selection in Ancient DNA"}} |
302 |
|
-**Source:** *bioRxiv Preprint* |
303 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *September 15, 2024* |
304 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Ali Akbari, Alison R. Barton, Steven Gazal, Zheng Li, Mohammadreza Kariminejad, et al.* |
305 |
|
-**Title:** *"Pervasive findings of directional selection realize the promise of ancient DNA to elucidate human adaptation"* |
306 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1101/2024.09.14.613021](https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.09.14.613021) |
307 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Genomics, Evolutionary Biology, Natural Selection* |
308 |
|
- |
309 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
310 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
311 |
|
- - Study analyzes **8,433 ancient individuals** from the past **14,000 years**. |
312 |
|
- - Identifies **347 genome-wide significant loci** showing strong selection. |
313 |
|
- |
314 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
315 |
|
- - Examines **West Eurasian populations** and their genetic evolution. |
316 |
|
- - Tracks **changes in allele frequencies over millennia**. |
317 |
|
- |
318 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
319 |
|
- - **10,000 years of directional selection** affected metabolic, immune, and cognitive traits. |
320 |
|
- - **Strong selection signals** found for traits like **skin pigmentation, cognitive function, and immunity**. |
321 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
322 |
|
- |
323 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
324 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
325 |
|
- - **Hundreds of alleles have been subject to directional selection** over recent millennia. |
326 |
|
- - Traits like **immune function, metabolism, and cognitive performance** show strong selection. |
327 |
|
- |
328 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
329 |
|
- - Selection pressure on **energy storage genes** supports the **Thrifty Gene Hypothesis**. |
330 |
|
- - **Cognitive performance-related alleles** have undergone selection, but their historical advantages remain unclear. |
331 |
|
- |
332 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
333 |
|
- - **Celiac disease risk allele** increased from **0% to 20%** in 4,000 years. |
334 |
|
- - **Blood type B frequency rose from 0% to 8% in 6,000 years**. |
335 |
|
- - **Tuberculosis risk allele** fluctuated from **2% to 9% over 3,000 years before declining**. |
336 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
337 |
|
- |
338 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
339 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
340 |
|
- - **Largest dataset to date** on natural selection in human ancient DNA. |
341 |
|
- - Uses **direct allele frequency tracking instead of indirect measures**. |
342 |
|
- |
343 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
344 |
|
- - Findings **may not translate directly** to modern populations. |
345 |
|
- - **Unclear whether observed selection pressures persist today**. |
346 |
|
- |
347 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
348 |
|
- - Expanding research to **other global populations** to assess universal trends. |
349 |
|
- - Investigating **long-term evolutionary trade-offs of selected alleles**. |
350 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
351 |
|
- |
352 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
353 |
|
-- Provides **direct evidence of long-term genetic adaptation** in human populations. |
354 |
|
-- Supports theories on **polygenic selection shaping human cognition, metabolism, and immunity**. |
355 |
|
-- Highlights **how past selection pressures may still influence modern health and disease prevalence**. |
356 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
357 |
|
- |
358 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
359 |
|
-1. Examine **selection patterns in non-European populations** for comparison. |
360 |
|
-2. Investigate **how environmental and cultural shifts influenced genetic selection**. |
361 |
|
-3. Explore **the genetic basis of traits linked to past and present-day human survival**. |
362 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
363 |
|
- |
364 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
365 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1101_2024.09.14.613021doi_.pdf]] |
366 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
367 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
368 |
|
- |
369 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age"}} |
370 |
|
-**Source:** *Twin Research and Human Genetics (Cambridge University Press)* |
371 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2013* |
372 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Thomas J. Bouchard Jr.* |
373 |
|
-**Title:** *"The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age"* |
374 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1017/thg.2013.54](https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2013.54) |
375 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Intelligence, Heritability, Developmental Psychology* |
376 |
|
- |
377 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
378 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
379 |
|
- - The study documents how the **heritability of IQ increases with age**, reaching an asymptote at **0.80 by adulthood**. |
380 |
|
- - Analysis is based on **longitudinal twin and adoption studies**. |
381 |
|
- |
382 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
383 |
|
- - Shared environmental influence on IQ **declines with age**, reaching **0.10 in adulthood**. |
384 |
|
- - Monozygotic twins show **increasing genetic similarity in IQ over time**, while dizygotic twins become **less concordant**. |
385 |
|
- |
386 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
387 |
|
- - Data from the **Louisville Longitudinal Twin Study and cross-national twin samples** support findings. |
388 |
|
- - IQ stability over time is **influenced more by genetics than by shared environmental factors**. |
389 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
390 |
|
- |
391 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
392 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
393 |
|
- - Intelligence heritability **strengthens throughout development**, contrary to early environmental models. |
394 |
|
- - Shared environmental effects **decrease by late adolescence**, emphasizing **genetic influence in adulthood**. |
395 |
|
- |
396 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
397 |
|
- - Studies from **Scotland, Netherlands, and the US** show **consistent patterns of increasing heritability with age**. |
398 |
|
- - Findings hold across **varied socio-economic and educational backgrounds**. |
399 |
|
- |
400 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
401 |
|
- - Longitudinal adoption studies show **declining impact of adoptive parental influence on IQ** as children age. |
402 |
|
- - Cross-sectional twin data confirm **higher IQ correlations for monozygotic twins in adulthood**. |
403 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
404 |
|
- |
405 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
406 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
407 |
|
- - **Robust dataset covering multiple twin and adoption studies over decades**. |
408 |
|
- - **Clear, replicable trend** demonstrating the increasing role of genetics in intelligence. |
409 |
|
- |
410 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
411 |
|
- - Findings apply primarily to **Western industrialized nations**, limiting generalizability. |
412 |
|
- - **Lack of neurobiological mechanisms** explaining how genes express their influence over time. |
413 |
|
- |
414 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
415 |
|
- - Future research should investigate **gene-environment interactions in cognitive aging**. |
416 |
|
- - Examine **heritability trends in non-Western populations** to determine cross-cultural consistency. |
417 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
418 |
|
- |
419 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
420 |
|
-- Provides **strong evidence for the genetic basis of intelligence**. |
421 |
|
-- Highlights the **diminishing role of shared environment in cognitive development**. |
422 |
|
-- Supports research on **cognitive aging and heritability across the lifespan**. |
423 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
424 |
|
- |
425 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
426 |
|
-1. Investigate **neurogenetic pathways underlying IQ development**. |
427 |
|
-2. Examine **how education and socioeconomic factors interact with genetic IQ influences**. |
428 |
|
-3. Study **heritability trends in aging populations and cognitive decline**. |
429 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
430 |
|
- |
431 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
432 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1017_thg.2013.54.pdf]] |
433 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
434 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
435 |
|
- |
436 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Is Homo sapiens polytypic? Human taxonomic diversity and its implications"}} |
437 |
|
-**Source:** *Medical Hypotheses (Elsevier)* |
438 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2010* |
439 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Michael A. Woodley* |
440 |
|
-**Title:** *"Is Homo sapiens polytypic? Human taxonomic diversity and its implications"* |
441 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1016/j.mehy.2009.07.046](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2009.07.046) |
442 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Human Taxonomy, Evolutionary Biology, Anthropology* |
443 |
|
- |
444 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
445 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
446 |
|
- - The study argues that **Homo sapiens is polytypic**, meaning it consists of multiple subspecies rather than a single monotypic species. |
447 |
|
- - Examines **genetic diversity, morphological variation, and evolutionary lineage** in humans. |
448 |
|
- |
449 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
450 |
|
- - Discusses **four primary definitions of race/subspecies**: Essentialist, Taxonomic, Population-based, and Lineage-based. |
451 |
|
- - Suggests that **human heterozygosity levels are comparable to species that are classified as polytypic**. |
452 |
|
- |
453 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
454 |
|
- - The study evaluates **FST values (genetic differentiation measure)** and argues that human genetic differentiation is comparable to that of recognized subspecies in other species. |
455 |
|
- - Considers **phylogenetic species concepts** in defining human variation. |
456 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
457 |
|
- |
458 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
459 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
460 |
|
- - Proposes that **modern human populations meet biological criteria for subspecies classification**. |
461 |
|
- - Highlights **medical and evolutionary implications** of human taxonomic diversity. |
462 |
|
- |
463 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
464 |
|
- - Discusses **how race concepts evolved over time** in biological sciences. |
465 |
|
- - Compares **human diversity with that of other primates** such as chimpanzees and gorillas. |
466 |
|
- |
467 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
468 |
|
- - Evaluates how **genetic markers correlate with population structure**. |
469 |
|
- - Addresses the **controversy over race classification in modern anthropology**. |
470 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
471 |
|
- |
472 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
473 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
474 |
|
- - Uses **comparative species analysis** to assess human classification. |
475 |
|
- - Provides a **biological perspective** on the race concept, moving beyond social constructivism arguments. |
476 |
|
- |
477 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
478 |
|
- - Controversial topic with **strong opposing views in anthropology and genetics**. |
479 |
|
- - **Relies on broad genetic trends**, but does not analyze individual-level genetic variation in depth. |
480 |
|
- |
481 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
482 |
|
- - Further research should **incorporate whole-genome studies** to refine subspecies classifications. |
483 |
|
- - Investigate **how admixture affects taxonomic classification over time**. |
484 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
485 |
|
- |
486 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
487 |
|
-- Contributes to discussions on **evolutionary taxonomy and species classification**. |
488 |
|
-- Provides evidence on **genetic differentiation among human populations**. |
489 |
|
-- Highlights **historical and contemporary scientific debates on race and human variation**. |
490 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
491 |
|
- |
492 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
493 |
|
-1. Examine **FST values in modern and ancient human populations**. |
494 |
|
-2. Investigate how **adaptive evolution influences population differentiation**. |
495 |
|
-3. Explore **the impact of genetic diversity on medical treatments and disease susceptibility**. |
496 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
497 |
|
- |
498 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
499 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.mehy.2009.07.046.pdf]] |
500 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
501 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
502 |
|
- |
503 |
|
-= IQ = |
504 |
|
- |
505 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence: Intelligence Research, Experts' Background, Controversial Issues, and the Media"}} |
506 |
|
-**Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)* |
507 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2019* |
508 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Heiner Rindermann, David Becker, Thomas R. Coyle* |
509 |
|
-**Title:** *"Survey of Expert Opinion on Intelligence: Intelligence Research, Experts' Background, Controversial Issues, and the Media"* |
510 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2019.101406](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2019.101406) |
511 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Psychology, Intelligence Research, Expert Analysis* |
512 |
|
- |
513 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
514 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
515 |
|
- - Survey of **102 experts** on intelligence research and public discourse. |
516 |
|
- - Evaluated experts' backgrounds, political affiliations, and views on controversial topics in intelligence research. |
517 |
|
- |
518 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
519 |
|
- - **90% of experts were from Western countries**, and **83% were male**. |
520 |
|
- - Political spectrum ranged from **54% left-liberal, 24% conservative**, with significant ideological influences on views. |
521 |
|
- |
522 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
523 |
|
- - Experts rated media coverage of intelligence research as **poor (avg. 3.1 on a 9-point scale)**. |
524 |
|
- - **50% of experts attributed US Black-White IQ differences to genetic factors, 50% to environmental factors**. |
525 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
526 |
|
- |
527 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
528 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
529 |
|
- - Experts overwhelmingly support **the g-factor theory of intelligence**. |
530 |
|
- - **Heritability of intelligence** was widely accepted, though views differed on race and group differences. |
531 |
|
- |
532 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
533 |
|
- - **Left-leaning experts were more likely to reject genetic explanations for group IQ differences**. |
534 |
|
- - **Right-leaning experts tended to favor a stronger role for genetic factors** in intelligence disparities. |
535 |
|
- |
536 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
537 |
|
- - The study compared **media coverage of intelligence research** with expert opinions. |
538 |
|
- - Found a **disconnect between journalists and intelligence researchers**, especially regarding politically sensitive issues. |
539 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
540 |
|
- |
541 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
542 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
543 |
|
- - **Largest expert survey on intelligence research** to date. |
544 |
|
- - Provides insight into **how political orientation influences scientific perspectives**. |
545 |
|
- |
546 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
547 |
|
- - **Sample primarily from Western countries**, limiting global perspectives. |
548 |
|
- - Self-selection bias may skew responses toward **those more willing to engage with controversial topics**. |
549 |
|
- |
550 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
551 |
|
- - Future studies should include **a broader range of global experts**. |
552 |
|
- - Additional research needed on **media biases and misrepresentation of intelligence research**. |
553 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
554 |
|
- |
555 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
556 |
|
-- Provides insight into **expert consensus and division on intelligence research**. |
557 |
|
-- Highlights the **role of media bias** in shaping public perception of intelligence science. |
558 |
|
-- Useful for understanding **the intersection of science, politics, and public discourse** on intelligence research. |
559 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
560 |
|
- |
561 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
562 |
|
-1. Examine **cross-national differences** in expert opinions on intelligence. |
563 |
|
-2. Investigate how **media bias impacts public understanding of intelligence research**. |
564 |
|
-3. Conduct follow-up studies with **a more diverse expert pool** to test findings. |
565 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
566 |
|
- |
567 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
568 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2019.101406.pdf]] |
569 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
570 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
571 |
|
- |
572 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: A Review of Intelligence GWAS Hits: Their Relationship to Country IQ and the Issue of Spatial Autocorrelation"}} |
573 |
|
-**Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)* |
574 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2015* |
575 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Davide Piffer* |
576 |
|
-**Title:** *"A Review of Intelligence GWAS Hits: Their Relationship to Country IQ and the Issue of Spatial Autocorrelation"* |
577 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2015.08.008](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.08.008) |
578 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Genetics, Intelligence, GWAS, Population Differences* |
579 |
|
- |
580 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
581 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
582 |
|
- - Study analyzed **genome-wide association studies (GWAS) hits** linked to intelligence. |
583 |
|
- - Found a **strong correlation (r = .91) between polygenic intelligence scores and national IQ levels**. |
584 |
|
- |
585 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
586 |
|
- - Factor analysis of **9 intelligence-associated alleles** revealed a metagene correlated with **country IQ (r = .86)**. |
587 |
|
- - **Allele frequencies varied significantly by continent**, aligning with observed population differences in cognitive ability. |
588 |
|
- |
589 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
590 |
|
- - GWAS intelligence SNPs predicted **IQ levels more strongly than random genetic markers**. |
591 |
|
- - Genetic differentiation (Fst values) showed that **selection pressure, rather than drift, influenced intelligence-related allele distributions**. |
592 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
593 |
|
- |
594 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
595 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
596 |
|
- - Intelligence-associated SNP frequencies correlate **highly with national IQ levels**. |
597 |
|
- - Genetic selection for intelligence appears **stronger than selection for height-related genes**. |
598 |
|
- |
599 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
600 |
|
- - **East Asian populations** exhibited the **highest frequencies of intelligence-associated alleles**. |
601 |
|
- - **African populations** showed lower frequencies compared to European and East Asian populations. |
602 |
|
- |
603 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
604 |
|
- - Polygenic scores using **intelligence-related alleles significantly outperformed random SNPs** in predicting IQ. |
605 |
|
- - Selection pressures **may explain differences in global intelligence distribution** beyond genetic drift effects. |
606 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
607 |
|
- |
608 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
609 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
610 |
|
- - **Comprehensive genetic analysis** of intelligence-linked SNPs. |
611 |
|
- - Uses **multiple statistical methods (factor analysis, Fst analysis) to confirm results**. |
612 |
|
- |
613 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
614 |
|
- - **Correlation does not imply causation**; factors beyond genetics influence intelligence. |
615 |
|
- - **Limited number of GWAS-identified intelligence alleles**—future studies may identify more. |
616 |
|
- |
617 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
618 |
|
- - Larger **cross-population GWAS studies** needed to validate findings. |
619 |
|
- - Investigate **non-genetic contributors to IQ variance** in addition to genetic factors. |
620 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
621 |
|
- |
622 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
623 |
|
-- Supports research on **genetic influences on intelligence at a population level**. |
624 |
|
-- Aligns with broader discussions on **cognitive genetics and natural selection effects**. |
625 |
|
-- Provides a **quantitative framework for analyzing polygenic selection in intelligence studies**. |
626 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
627 |
|
- |
628 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
629 |
|
-1. Conduct **expanded GWAS studies** including diverse populations. |
630 |
|
-2. Investigate **gene-environment interactions influencing intelligence**. |
631 |
|
-3. Explore **historical selection pressures shaping intelligence-related alleles**. |
632 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
633 |
|
- |
634 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
635 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2015.08.008.pdf]] |
636 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
637 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
638 |
|
- |
639 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Genetic Structure, Self-Identified Race/Ethnicity, and Confounding"}} |
640 |
|
-**Source:** Journal of Genetic Epidemiology |
641 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** 2024-01-15 |
642 |
|
-**Author(s):** Smith et al. |
643 |
|
-**Title:** "Genetic Structure, Self-Identified Race/Ethnicity, and Confounding in Case-Control Association Studies" |
644 |
|
-**DOI:** [https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235](https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235) |
645 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** Genetics, Social Science |
646 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
647 |
|
- |
648 |
|
-= Dating = |
649 |
|
- |
650 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Trends in Frequency of Sexual Activity and Number of Sexual Partners Among Adults Aged 18 to 44 Years in the US, 2000-2018"}} |
651 |
|
-**Source:** *JAMA Network Open* |
652 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2020* |
653 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Ueda P, Mercer CH, Ghaznavi C, Herbenick D.* |
654 |
|
-**Title:** *"Trends in Frequency of Sexual Activity and Number of Sexual Partners Among Adults Aged 18 to 44 Years in the US, 2000-2018"* |
655 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3833](https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3833) |
656 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Public Health, Sexual Behavior, Demography* |
657 |
|
- |
658 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
659 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
660 |
|
- - Study analyzed **General Social Survey (2000-2018)** data. |
661 |
|
- - Found **declining trends in sexual activity** among young adults. |
662 |
|
- |
663 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
664 |
|
- - Decreases in sexual activity were most prominent among **men aged 18-34**. |
665 |
|
- - Factors like **marital status, employment, and psychological well-being** were associated with changes in sexual frequency. |
666 |
|
- |
667 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
668 |
|
- - Frequency of sexual activity decreased by **8-10%** over the studied period. |
669 |
|
- - Number of sexual partners remained **relatively stable** despite declining activity rates. |
670 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
671 |
|
- |
672 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
673 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
674 |
|
- - A significant decline in sexual frequency, especially among **younger men**. |
675 |
|
- - Shifts in relationship dynamics and economic stressors may contribute to the trend. |
676 |
|
- |
677 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
678 |
|
- - More pronounced decline among **unmarried individuals**. |
679 |
|
- - No major change observed for **married adults** over time. |
680 |
|
- |
681 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
682 |
|
- - **Mental health and employment status** were correlated with decreased activity. |
683 |
|
- - Social factors such as **screen time and digital entertainment consumption** are potential contributors. |
684 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
685 |
|
- |
686 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
687 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
688 |
|
- - **Large sample size** from a nationally representative dataset. |
689 |
|
- - **Longitudinal design** enables trend analysis over time. |
690 |
|
- |
691 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
692 |
|
- - Self-reported data may introduce **response bias**. |
693 |
|
- - No direct causal mechanisms tested for the decline in sexual activity. |
694 |
|
- |
695 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
696 |
|
- - Further studies should incorporate **qualitative data** on behavioral shifts. |
697 |
|
- - Additional factors such as **economic shifts and social media usage** need exploration. |
698 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
699 |
|
- |
700 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
701 |
|
-- Provides evidence on **changing demographic behaviors** in relation to relationships and social interactions. |
702 |
|
-- Highlights the role of **mental health, employment, and societal changes** in personal behaviors. |
703 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
704 |
|
- |
705 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
706 |
|
-1. Investigate the **impact of digital media consumption** on relationship dynamics. |
707 |
|
-2. Examine **regional and cultural differences** in sexual activity trends. |
708 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
709 |
|
- |
710 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
711 |
711 |
|
712 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
713 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
714 |
|
- |
715 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Biracial Couples and Adverse Birth Outcomes – A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis"}} |
716 |
|
-**Source:** *Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica* |
717 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2012* |
718 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Ravisha M. Srinivasjois, Shreya Shah, Prakesh S. Shah, Knowledge Synthesis Group on Determinants of Preterm/LBW Births* |
719 |
|
-**Title:** *"Biracial Couples and Adverse Birth Outcomes: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis"* |
720 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1111/j.1600-0412.2012.01501.x](https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0412.2012.01501.x) |
721 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Neonatal Health, Maternal-Fetal Medicine, Racial Disparities* |
722 |
|
- |
723 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
724 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
725 |
|
- - Meta-analysis of **26,335,596 singleton births** from eight studies. |
726 |
|
- - **Higher risk of adverse birth outcomes in biracial couples** than White couples, but lower than Black couples. |
727 |
|
- |
728 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
729 |
|
- - **Maternal race had a stronger influence than paternal race** on birth outcomes. |
730 |
|
- - **Black mother–White father (BMWF) couples** had a higher risk than **White mother–Black father (WMBF) couples**. |
731 |
|
- |
732 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
733 |
|
- - **Adjusted Odds Ratios (aORs) for key outcomes:** |
734 |
|
- - **Low birthweight (LBW):** WMBF (1.21), BMWF (1.75), Black mother–Black father (BMBF) (2.08). |
735 |
|
- - **Preterm births (PTB):** WMBF (1.17), BMWF (1.37), BMBF (1.78). |
736 |
|
- - **Stillbirths:** WMBF (1.43), BMWF (1.51), BMBF (1.85). |
737 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
738 |
|
- |
739 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
740 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
741 |
|
- - **Biracial couples face a gradient of risk**: higher than White couples but lower than Black couples. |
742 |
|
- - **Maternal race plays a more significant role** in pregnancy outcomes. |
743 |
|
- |
744 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
745 |
|
- - **Black mothers (regardless of paternal race) had the highest risk of LBW and PTB**. |
746 |
|
- - **White mothers with Black fathers had a lower risk** than Black mothers with White fathers. |
747 |
|
- |
748 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
749 |
|
- - The **weathering hypothesis** suggests that **long-term stress exposure** contributes to higher adverse birth risks in Black mothers. |
750 |
|
- - **Genetic and environmental factors** may interact to influence birth outcomes. |
751 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
752 |
|
- |
753 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
754 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
755 |
|
- - **Largest meta-analysis** on racial disparities in birth outcomes. |
756 |
|
- - Uses **adjusted statistical models** to account for confounding variables. |
757 |
|
- |
758 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
759 |
|
- - Data limited to **Black-White biracial couples**, excluding other racial groups. |
760 |
|
- - **Socioeconomic and healthcare access factors** not fully explored. |
761 |
|
- |
762 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
763 |
|
- - Future studies should examine **Asian, Hispanic, and Indigenous biracial couples**. |
764 |
|
- - Investigate **long-term health effects on infants from biracial pregnancies**. |
765 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
766 |
|
- |
767 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
768 |
|
-- Provides **critical insights into racial disparities** in maternal and infant health. |
769 |
|
-- Supports **research on genetic and environmental influences on neonatal health**. |
770 |
|
-- Highlights **how maternal race plays a more significant role than paternal race** in birth outcomes. |
771 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
772 |
|
- |
773 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
774 |
|
-1. Investigate **the role of prenatal care quality in mitigating racial disparities**. |
775 |
|
-2. Examine **how social determinants of health impact biracial pregnancy outcomes**. |
776 |
|
-3. Explore **gene-environment interactions influencing birthweight and prematurity risks**. |
777 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
778 |
|
- |
779 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
780 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1111_j.1600-0412.2012.01501.xAbstract.pdf]] |
781 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
782 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
783 |
|
- |
784 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: One is the Loneliest Number: Involuntary Celibacy (Incel), Mental Health, and Loneliness"}} |
785 |
|
-**Source:** *Current Psychology* |
786 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2024* |
787 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Brandon Sparks, Alexandra M. Zidenberg, Mark E. Olver* |
788 |
|
-**Title:** *"One is the Loneliest Number: Involuntary Celibacy (Incel), Mental Health, and Loneliness"* |
789 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1007/s12144-023-04275-z](https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04275-z) |
790 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Psychology, Mental Health, Social Isolation* |
791 |
|
- |
792 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
793 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
794 |
|
- - Study analyzed **67 self-identified incels** and **103 non-incel men**. |
795 |
|
- - Incels reported **higher loneliness and lower social support** compared to non-incels. |
796 |
|
- |
797 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
798 |
|
- - Incels exhibited **higher levels of depression, anxiety, and self-critical rumination**. |
799 |
|
- - **Social isolation was a key factor** differentiating incels from non-incels. |
800 |
|
- |
801 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
802 |
|
- - 95% of incels in the study reported **having depression**, with 38% receiving a formal diagnosis. |
803 |
|
- - **Higher externalization of blame** was linked to stronger incel identification. |
804 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
805 |
|
- |
806 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
807 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
808 |
|
- - Incels experience **heightened rejection sensitivity and loneliness**. |
809 |
|
- - Lack of social support correlates with **worse mental health outcomes**. |
810 |
|
- |
811 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
812 |
|
- - **Avoidant attachment styles** were a strong predictor of incel identity. |
813 |
|
- - **Mate value perceptions** significantly differed between incels and non-incels. |
814 |
|
- |
815 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
816 |
|
- - Incels **engaged in fewer positive coping mechanisms** such as emotional support or positive reframing. |
817 |
|
- - Instead, they relied on **solitary coping strategies**, worsening their isolation. |
818 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
819 |
|
- |
820 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
821 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
822 |
|
- - **First quantitative study** on incels’ social isolation and mental health. |
823 |
|
- - **Robust sample size** and validated psychological measures. |
824 |
|
- |
825 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
826 |
|
- - Sample drawn from **Reddit communities**, which may not represent all incels. |
827 |
|
- - **No causal conclusions**—correlations between isolation and inceldom need further research. |
828 |
|
- |
829 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
830 |
|
- - Future studies should **compare incel forum users vs. non-users**. |
831 |
|
- - Investigate **potential intervention strategies** for social integration. |
832 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
833 |
|
- |
834 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
835 |
|
-- Highlights **mental health vulnerabilities** within the incel community. |
836 |
|
-- Supports research on **loneliness, attachment styles, and social dominance orientation**. |
837 |
|
-- Examines how **peer rejection influences self-perceived mate value**. |
838 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
839 |
|
- |
840 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
841 |
|
-1. Explore how **online community participation** affects incel mental health. |
842 |
|
-2. Investigate **cognitive biases** influencing self-perceived rejection among incels. |
843 |
|
-3. Assess **therapeutic interventions** to address incel social isolation. |
844 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
845 |
|
- |
846 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
847 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1007_s12144-023-04275-z.pdf]] |
848 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
849 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
850 |
|
- |
851 |
|
-= Crime and Substance Abuse = |
852 |
|
- |
853 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"}} |
854 |
|
-**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
855 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2002* |
856 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Clifford A. Butzin, Christine A. Saum, Frank R. Scarpitti* |
857 |
|
-**Title:** *"Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"* |
858 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120014424](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120014424) |
859 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Substance Use, Criminal Justice, Drug Courts* |
860 |
|
- |
861 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
862 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
863 |
|
- - Study examined **drug treatment court success rates** among first-time offenders. |
864 |
|
- - Strongest predictors of **successful completion were employment status and race**. |
865 |
|
- |
866 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
867 |
|
- - Individuals with **stable jobs were more likely to complete the program**. |
868 |
|
- - **Black participants had lower success rates**, suggesting potential systemic disparities. |
869 |
|
- |
870 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
871 |
|
- - **Education level was positively correlated** with program completion. |
872 |
|
- - Frequency of **drug use before enrollment affected treatment outcomes**. |
873 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
874 |
|
- |
875 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
876 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
877 |
|
- - **Social stability factors** (employment, education) were key to treatment success. |
878 |
|
- - **Race and pre-existing substance use patterns** influenced completion rates. |
879 |
|
- |
880 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
881 |
|
- - White offenders had **higher completion rates** than Black offenders. |
882 |
|
- - Drug court success was **higher for those with lower initial drug use frequency**. |
883 |
|
- |
884 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
885 |
|
- - **Individuals with strong social ties were more likely to finish the program**. |
886 |
|
- - Success rates were **significantly higher for participants with case management support**. |
887 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
888 |
|
- |
889 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
890 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
891 |
|
- - **First empirical study on drug court program success factors**. |
892 |
|
- - Uses **longitudinal data** for post-treatment analysis. |
893 |
|
- |
894 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
895 |
|
- - Lacks **qualitative data on personal motivation and treatment engagement**. |
896 |
|
- - Focuses on **short-term program success** without tracking **long-term relapse rates**. |
897 |
|
- |
898 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
899 |
|
- - Future research should examine **racial disparities in drug court outcomes**. |
900 |
|
- - Study **how community resources impact long-term recovery**. |
901 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
902 |
|
- |
903 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
904 |
|
-- Provides insight into **what factors contribute to drug court program success**. |
905 |
|
-- Highlights **racial disparities in criminal justice-based rehabilitation programs**. |
906 |
|
-- Supports **policy discussions on improving access to drug treatment for marginalized groups**. |
907 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
908 |
|
- |
909 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
910 |
|
-1. Investigate **the role of mental health in drug court success rates**. |
911 |
|
-2. Assess **long-term relapse prevention strategies post-treatment**. |
912 |
|
-3. Explore **alternative diversion programs beyond traditional drug courts**. |
913 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
914 |
|
- |
915 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
916 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120014424.pdf]] |
917 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
918 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
919 |
|
- |
920 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys"}} |
921 |
|
-**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
922 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2003* |
923 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Timothy P. Johnson, Phillip J. Bowman* |
924 |
|
-**Title:** *"Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys"* |
925 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120023394](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120023394) |
926 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Survey Methodology, Racial Disparities, Substance Use Research* |
927 |
|
- |
928 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
929 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
930 |
|
- - Study examined **how racial and cultural factors influence self-reported substance use data**. |
931 |
|
- - Analyzed **36 empirical studies from 1977–2003** on survey reliability across racial/ethnic groups. |
932 |
|
- |
933 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
934 |
|
- - Black and Latino respondents **were more likely to underreport drug use** compared to White respondents. |
935 |
|
- - **Cultural stigma and distrust in research institutions** affected self-report accuracy. |
936 |
|
- |
937 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
938 |
|
- - **Surveys using biological validation (urinalysis, hair tests) revealed underreporting trends**. |
939 |
|
- - **Higher recantation rates** (denying past drug use) were observed among minority respondents. |
940 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
941 |
|
- |
942 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
943 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
944 |
|
- - Racial/ethnic disparities in **substance use reporting bias survey-based research**. |
945 |
|
- - **Social desirability and cultural norms impact data reliability**. |
946 |
|
- |
947 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
948 |
|
- - White respondents were **more likely to overreport** substance use. |
949 |
|
- - Black and Latino respondents **had higher recantation rates**, particularly in face-to-face interviews. |
950 |
|
- |
951 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
952 |
|
- - Mode of survey administration **significantly influenced reporting accuracy**. |
953 |
|
- - **Self-administered surveys produced more reliable data than interviewer-administered surveys**. |
954 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
955 |
|
- |
956 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
957 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
958 |
|
- - **Comprehensive review of 36 studies** on measurement error in substance use reporting. |
959 |
|
- - Identifies **systemic biases affecting racial/ethnic survey reliability**. |
960 |
|
- |
961 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
962 |
|
- - Relies on **secondary data analysis**, limiting direct experimental control. |
963 |
|
- - Does not explore **how measurement error impacts policy decisions**. |
964 |
|
- |
965 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
966 |
|
- - Future research should **incorporate mixed-method approaches** (qualitative & quantitative). |
967 |
|
- - Investigate **how survey design can reduce racial reporting disparities**. |
968 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
969 |
|
- |
970 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
971 |
|
-- Supports research on **racial disparities in self-reported health behaviors**. |
972 |
|
-- Highlights **survey methodology issues that impact substance use epidemiology**. |
973 |
|
-- Provides insights for **improving data accuracy in public health research**. |
974 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
975 |
|
- |
976 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
977 |
|
-1. Investigate **how survey design impacts racial disparities in self-reported health data**. |
978 |
|
-2. Study **alternative data collection methods (biometric validation, passive data tracking)**. |
979 |
|
-3. Explore **the role of social stigma in self-reported health behaviors**. |
980 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
981 |
|
- |
982 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
983 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120023394.pdf]] |
984 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
985 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
986 |
|
- |
987 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"}} |
988 |
|
-**Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* |
989 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2002* |
990 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Clifford A. Butzin, Christine A. Saum, Frank R. Scarpitti* |
991 |
|
-**Title:** *"Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"* |
992 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120014424](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120014424) |
993 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Substance Use, Criminal Justice, Drug Courts* |
994 |
|
- |
995 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
996 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
997 |
|
- - Study examined **drug treatment court success rates** among first-time offenders. |
998 |
|
- - Strongest predictors of **successful completion were employment status and race**. |
999 |
|
- |
1000 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1001 |
|
- - Individuals with **stable jobs were more likely to complete the program**. |
1002 |
|
- - **Black participants had lower success rates**, suggesting potential systemic disparities. |
1003 |
|
- |
1004 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1005 |
|
- - **Education level was positively correlated** with program completion. |
1006 |
|
- - Frequency of **drug use before enrollment affected treatment outcomes**. |
1007 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1008 |
|
- |
1009 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
1010 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1011 |
|
- - **Social stability factors** (employment, education) were key to treatment success. |
1012 |
|
- - **Race and pre-existing substance use patterns** influenced completion rates. |
1013 |
|
- |
1014 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1015 |
|
- - White offenders had **higher completion rates** than Black offenders. |
1016 |
|
- - Drug court success was **higher for those with lower initial drug use frequency**. |
1017 |
|
- |
1018 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1019 |
|
- - **Individuals with strong social ties were more likely to finish the program**. |
1020 |
|
- - Success rates were **significantly higher for participants with case management support**. |
1021 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1022 |
|
- |
1023 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
1024 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1025 |
|
- - **First empirical study on drug court program success factors**. |
1026 |
|
- - Uses **longitudinal data** for post-treatment analysis. |
1027 |
|
- |
1028 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1029 |
|
- - Lacks **qualitative data on personal motivation and treatment engagement**. |
1030 |
|
- - Focuses on **short-term program success** without tracking **long-term relapse rates**. |
1031 |
|
- |
1032 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1033 |
|
- - Future research should examine **racial disparities in drug court outcomes**. |
1034 |
|
- - Study **how community resources impact long-term recovery**. |
1035 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1036 |
|
- |
1037 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
1038 |
|
-- Provides insight into **what factors contribute to drug court program success**. |
1039 |
|
-- Highlights **racial disparities in criminal justice-based rehabilitation programs**. |
1040 |
|
-- Supports **policy discussions on improving access to drug treatment for marginalized groups**. |
1041 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1042 |
|
- |
1043 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
1044 |
|
-1. Investigate **the role of mental health in drug court success rates**. |
1045 |
|
-2. Assess **long-term relapse prevention strategies post-treatment**. |
1046 |
|
-3. Explore **alternative diversion programs beyond traditional drug courts**. |
1047 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1048 |
|
- |
1049 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
1050 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120014424.pdf]] |
1051 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1052 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1053 |
|
- |
1054 |
|
-{{expandable summary=" |
1055 |
|
- |
1056 |
|
-Study: Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time?"}} |
1057 |
|
-**Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)* |
1058 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2014* |
1059 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Michael A. Woodley, Jan te Nijenhuis, Raegan Murphy* |
1060 |
|
-**Title:** *"Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time?"* |
1061 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.012](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.012) |
1062 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Cognitive Decline, Intelligence, Dysgenics* |
1063 |
|
- |
1064 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
1065 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1066 |
|
- - The study examines reaction time data from **13 age-matched studies** spanning **1884–2004**. |
1067 |
|
- - Results suggest an estimated **decline of 13.35 IQ points** over this period. |
1068 |
|
- |
1069 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1070 |
|
- - The study found **slower reaction times in modern populations** compared to Victorian-era individuals. |
1071 |
|
- - Data from **Western countries (US, UK, Canada, Australia, Finland)** were analyzed. |
1072 |
|
- |
1073 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1074 |
|
- - The estimated **dysgenic rate is 1.21 IQ points lost per decade**. |
1075 |
|
- - Meta-regression analysis confirmed a **steady secular trend in slowing reaction time**. |
1076 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1077 |
|
- |
1078 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
1079 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1080 |
|
- - Supports the hypothesis of **intelligence decline due to genetic and environmental factors**. |
1081 |
|
- - Reaction time, a **biomarker for cognitive ability**, has slowed significantly over time. |
1082 |
|
- |
1083 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1084 |
|
- - A stronger **correlation between slower reaction time and lower general intelligence (g)**. |
1085 |
|
- - Flynn effect (IQ gains) does not contradict this finding, as reaction time is a **biological, not environmental, measure**. |
1086 |
|
- |
1087 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1088 |
|
- - Cross-national comparisons indicate a **global trend in slower reaction times**. |
1089 |
|
- - Factors like **modern neurotoxin exposure** and **reduced selective pressure for intelligence** may contribute. |
1090 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1091 |
|
- |
1092 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
1093 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1094 |
|
- - **Comprehensive meta-analysis** covering over a century of reaction time data. |
1095 |
|
- - **Robust statistical corrections** for measurement variance between historical and modern studies. |
1096 |
|
- |
1097 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1098 |
|
- - Some historical data sources **lack methodological consistency**. |
1099 |
|
- - **Reaction time measurements vary by study**, requiring adjustments for equipment differences. |
1100 |
|
- |
1101 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1102 |
|
- - Future studies should **replicate results with more modern datasets**. |
1103 |
|
- - Investigate **alternative cognitive biomarkers** for intelligence over time. |
1104 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1105 |
|
- |
1106 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
1107 |
|
-- Provides evidence for **long-term intelligence trends**, contributing to research on **cognitive evolution**. |
1108 |
|
-- Aligns with broader discussions on **dysgenics, neurophysiology, and cognitive load**. |
1109 |
|
-- Supports the argument that **modern societies may be experiencing intelligence decline**. |
1110 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1111 |
|
- |
1112 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
1113 |
|
-1. Investigate **genetic markers associated with reaction time** and intelligence decline. |
1114 |
|
-2. Examine **regional variations in reaction time trends**. |
1115 |
|
-3. Explore **cognitive resilience factors that counteract the decline**. |
1116 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1117 |
|
- |
1118 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
1119 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2014.05.012.pdf]] |
1120 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1121 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1122 |
|
- |
1123 |
|
-= Whiteness & White Guilt = |
1124 |
|
- |
1125 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Segregation, Innocence, and Protection: The Institutional Conditions That Maintain Whiteness in College Sports"}} |
1126 |
|
-**Source:** *Journal of Diversity in Higher Education* |
1127 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2019* |
1128 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Kirsten Hextrum* |
1129 |
|
-**Title:** *"Segregation, Innocence, and Protection: The Institutional Conditions That Maintain Whiteness in College Sports"* |
1130 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1037/dhe0000140](https://doi.org/10.1037/dhe0000140) |
1131 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Race and Sports, Higher Education, Institutional Racism* |
1132 |
|
- |
1133 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
1134 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1135 |
|
- - Analyzed **47 college athlete narratives** to explore racial disparities in non-revenue sports. |
1136 |
|
- - Found three interrelated themes: **racial segregation, racial innocence, and racial protection**. |
1137 |
|
- |
1138 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1139 |
|
- - **Predominantly white sports programs** reinforce racial hierarchies in college athletics. |
1140 |
|
- - **Recruitment policies favor white athletes** from affluent, suburban backgrounds. |
1141 |
|
- |
1142 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1143 |
|
- - White athletes are **socialized to remain unaware of racial privilege** in their athletic careers. |
1144 |
|
- - Media and institutional narratives protect white athletes from discussions on race and systemic inequities. |
1145 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1146 |
|
- |
1147 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
1148 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1149 |
|
- - Colleges **actively recruit white athletes** from majority-white communities. |
1150 |
|
- - Institutional policies **uphold whiteness** by failing to challenge racial biases in recruitment and team culture. |
1151 |
|
- |
1152 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1153 |
|
- - **White athletes show limited awareness** of their racial advantage in sports. |
1154 |
|
- - **Black athletes are overrepresented** in revenue-generating sports but underrepresented in non-revenue teams. |
1155 |
|
- |
1156 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1157 |
|
- - Examines **how sports serve as a mechanism for maintaining racial privilege** in higher education. |
1158 |
|
- - Discusses the **role of athletics in reinforcing systemic segregation and exclusion**. |
1159 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1160 |
|
- |
1161 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
1162 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1163 |
|
- - **Comprehensive qualitative analysis** of race in college sports. |
1164 |
|
- - Examines **institutional conditions** that sustain racial disparities in athletics. |
1165 |
|
- |
1166 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1167 |
|
- - Focuses primarily on **Division I non-revenue sports**, limiting generalizability to other divisions. |
1168 |
|
- - Lacks extensive **quantitative data on racial demographics** in college athletics. |
1169 |
|
- |
1170 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1171 |
|
- - Future research should **compare recruitment policies across different sports and divisions**. |
1172 |
|
- - Investigate **how athletic scholarships contribute to racial inequities in higher education**. |
1173 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1174 |
|
- |
1175 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
1176 |
|
-- Provides evidence of **systemic racial biases** in college sports recruitment. |
1177 |
|
-- Highlights **how institutional policies protect whiteness** in non-revenue athletics. |
1178 |
|
-- Supports research on **diversity, equity, and inclusion (DEI) efforts in sports and education**. |
1179 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1180 |
|
- |
1181 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
1182 |
|
-1. Investigate how **racial stereotypes influence college athlete recruitment**. |
1183 |
|
-2. Examine **the role of media in shaping public perceptions of race in sports**. |
1184 |
|
-3. Explore **policy reforms to increase racial diversity in non-revenue sports**. |
1185 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1186 |
|
- |
1187 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
1188 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1037_dhe0000140.pdf]] |
1189 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1190 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1191 |
|
- |
1192 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Racial Bias in Pain Assessment and Treatment Recommendations"}} |
1193 |
|
-**Source:** *Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS)* |
1194 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2016* |
1195 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Kelly M. Hoffman, Sophie Trawalter, Jordan R. Axta, M. Norman Oliver* |
1196 |
|
-**Title:** *"Racial Bias in Pain Assessment and Treatment Recommendations, and False Beliefs About Biological Differences Between Blacks and Whites"* |
1197 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1073/pnas.1516047113](https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1516047113) |
1198 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Health Disparities, Racial Bias, Medical Treatment* |
1199 |
|
- |
1200 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
1201 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1202 |
|
- - Study analyzed **racial disparities in pain perception and treatment recommendations**. |
1203 |
|
- - Found that **white laypeople and medical students endorsed false beliefs about biological differences** between Black and white individuals. |
1204 |
|
- |
1205 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1206 |
|
- - **50% of medical students surveyed endorsed at least one false belief about biological differences**. |
1207 |
|
- - Participants who held these false beliefs were **more likely to underestimate Black patients’ pain levels**. |
1208 |
|
- |
1209 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1210 |
|
- - **Black patients were less likely to receive appropriate pain treatment** compared to white patients. |
1211 |
|
- - The study confirmed that **historical misconceptions about racial differences still persist in modern medicine**. |
1212 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1213 |
|
- |
1214 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
1215 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1216 |
|
- - False beliefs about biological racial differences **correlate with racial disparities in pain treatment**. |
1217 |
|
- - Medical students and residents who endorsed these beliefs **showed greater racial bias in treatment recommendations**. |
1218 |
|
- |
1219 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1220 |
|
- - Physicians who **did not endorse these beliefs** showed **no racial bias** in treatment recommendations. |
1221 |
|
- - Bias was **strongest among first-year medical students** and decreased slightly in later years of training. |
1222 |
|
- |
1223 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1224 |
|
- - Study participants **underestimated Black patients' pain and recommended less effective pain treatments**. |
1225 |
|
- - The study suggests that **racial disparities in medical care stem, in part, from these enduring false beliefs**. |
1226 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1227 |
|
- |
1228 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
1229 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1230 |
|
- - **First empirical study to connect false racial beliefs with medical decision-making**. |
1231 |
|
- - Utilizes a **large sample of medical students and residents** from diverse institutions. |
1232 |
|
- |
1233 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1234 |
|
- - The study focuses on **Black vs. white disparities**, leaving other racial/ethnic groups unexplored. |
1235 |
|
- - Participants' responses were based on **hypothetical medical cases, not real-world treatment decisions**. |
1236 |
|
- |
1237 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1238 |
|
- - Future research should examine **how these biases manifest in real clinical settings**. |
1239 |
|
- - Investigate **whether medical training can correct these biases over time**. |
1240 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1241 |
|
- |
1242 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
1243 |
|
-- Highlights **racial disparities in healthcare**, specifically in pain assessment and treatment. |
1244 |
|
-- Supports **research on implicit bias and its impact on medical outcomes**. |
1245 |
|
-- Provides evidence for **the need to address racial bias in medical education**. |
1246 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1247 |
|
- |
1248 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
1249 |
|
-1. Investigate **interventions to reduce racial bias in medical decision-making**. |
1250 |
|
-2. Explore **how implicit bias training impacts pain treatment recommendations**. |
1251 |
|
-3. Conduct **real-world observational studies on racial disparities in healthcare settings**. |
1252 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1253 |
|
- |
1254 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
1255 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1073_pnas.1516047113.pdf]] |
1256 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1257 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1258 |
|
- |
1259 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Rising Morbidity and Mortality in Midlife Among White Non-Hispanic Americans"}} |
1260 |
|
-**Source:** *Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS)* |
1261 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2015* |
1262 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Anne Case, Angus Deaton* |
1263 |
|
-**Title:** *"Rising Morbidity and Mortality in Midlife Among White Non-Hispanic Americans in the 21st Century"* |
1264 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1073/pnas.1518393112](https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1518393112) |
1265 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Public Health, Mortality, Socioeconomic Factors* |
1266 |
|
- |
1267 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
1268 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1269 |
|
- - Mortality rates among **middle-aged white non-Hispanic Americans (ages 45–54)** increased from 1999 to 2013. |
1270 |
|
- - This reversal in mortality trends is unique to the U.S.; **no other wealthy country experienced a similar rise**. |
1271 |
|
- |
1272 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1273 |
|
- - The increase was **most pronounced among those with a high school education or less**. |
1274 |
|
- - Hispanic and Black non-Hispanic mortality continued to decline over the same period. |
1275 |
|
- |
1276 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1277 |
|
- - Rising mortality was driven primarily by **suicide, drug and alcohol poisoning, and chronic liver disease**. |
1278 |
|
- - Midlife morbidity increased as well, with more reports of **poor health, pain, and mental distress**. |
1279 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1280 |
|
- |
1281 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
1282 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1283 |
|
- - The rise in mortality is attributed to **substance abuse, economic distress, and deteriorating mental health**. |
1284 |
|
- - The increase in **suicides and opioid overdoses parallels broader socioeconomic decline**. |
1285 |
|
- |
1286 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1287 |
|
- - The **largest mortality increases** occurred among **whites without a college degree**. |
1288 |
|
- - Chronic pain, functional limitations, and self-reported mental distress **rose significantly in affected groups**. |
1289 |
|
- |
1290 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1291 |
|
- - **Educational attainment was a major predictor of mortality trends**, with better-educated individuals experiencing lower mortality rates. |
1292 |
|
- - Mortality among **white Americans with a college degree continued to decline**, resembling trends in other wealthy nations. |
1293 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1294 |
|
- |
1295 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
1296 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1297 |
|
- - **First major study to highlight rising midlife mortality among U.S. whites**. |
1298 |
|
- - Uses **CDC and Census mortality data spanning over a decade**. |
1299 |
|
- |
1300 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1301 |
|
- - Does not establish **causality** between economic decline and increased mortality. |
1302 |
|
- - Lacks **granular data on opioid prescribing patterns and regional differences**. |
1303 |
|
- |
1304 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1305 |
|
- - Future studies should explore **how economic shifts, healthcare access, and mental health treatment contribute to these trends**. |
1306 |
|
- - Further research on **racial and socioeconomic disparities in mortality trends** is needed. |
1307 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1308 |
|
- |
1309 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
1310 |
|
-- Highlights **socioeconomic and racial disparities** in health outcomes. |
1311 |
|
-- Supports research on **substance abuse and mental health crises in the U.S.**. |
1312 |
|
-- Provides evidence for **the role of economic instability in public health trends**. |
1313 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1314 |
|
- |
1315 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
1316 |
|
-1. Investigate **regional differences in rising midlife mortality**. |
1317 |
|
-2. Examine the **impact of the opioid crisis on long-term health trends**. |
1318 |
|
-3. Study **policy interventions aimed at reversing rising mortality rates**. |
1319 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1320 |
|
- |
1321 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
1322 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1073_pnas.1518393112.pdf]] |
1323 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1324 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1325 |
|
- |
1326 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: How Do People Without Migration Background Experience and Impact Today’s Superdiverse Cities?"}} |
1327 |
|
-**Source:** *Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies* |
1328 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2023* |
1329 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Maurice Crul, Frans Lelie, Elif Keskiner, Laure Michon, Ismintha Waldring* |
1330 |
|
-**Title:** *"How Do People Without Migration Background Experience and Impact Today’s Superdiverse Cities?"* |
1331 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1080/1369183X.2023.2182548](https://doi.org/10.1080/1369183X.2023.2182548) |
1332 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Urban Sociology, Migration Studies, Integration* |
1333 |
|
- |
1334 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
1335 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1336 |
|
- - Study examines the role of **people without migration background** in majority-minority cities. |
1337 |
|
- - Analyzes **over 3,000 survey responses and 150 in-depth interviews** from six North-Western European cities. |
1338 |
|
- |
1339 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1340 |
|
- - Explores differences in **integration, social interactions, and perceptions of diversity**. |
1341 |
|
- - Studies how **class, education, and neighborhood composition** affect adaptation to urban diversity. |
1342 |
|
- |
1343 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1344 |
|
- - The study introduces the **Becoming a Minority (BaM) project**, a large-scale investigation of urban demographic shifts. |
1345 |
|
- - **People without migration background perceive diversity differently**, with some embracing and others resisting change. |
1346 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1347 |
|
- |
1348 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
1349 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1350 |
|
- - The study **challenges traditional integration theories**, arguing that non-migrant groups also undergo adaptation processes. |
1351 |
|
- - Some residents **struggle with demographic changes**, while others see diversity as an asset. |
1352 |
|
- |
1353 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1354 |
|
- - Young, educated individuals in urban areas **are more open to cultural diversity**. |
1355 |
|
- - Older and less mobile residents **report feelings of displacement and social isolation**. |
1356 |
|
- |
1357 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1358 |
|
- - Examines how **people without migration background navigate majority-minority settings** in cities like Amsterdam and Vienna. |
1359 |
|
- - Analyzes **whether former ethnic majority groups now perceive themselves as minorities**. |
1360 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1361 |
|
- |
1362 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
1363 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1364 |
|
- - **Innovative approach** by examining the impact of migration on native populations. |
1365 |
|
- - Uses **both qualitative and quantitative data** for robust analysis. |
1366 |
|
- |
1367 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1368 |
|
- - Limited to **Western European urban settings**, missing perspectives from other global regions. |
1369 |
|
- - Does not fully explore **policy interventions for fostering social cohesion**. |
1370 |
|
- |
1371 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1372 |
|
- - Expand research to **other geographical contexts** to understand migration effects globally. |
1373 |
|
- - Investigate **long-term trends in urban adaptation and community building**. |
1374 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1375 |
|
- |
1376 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
1377 |
|
-- Provides a **new perspective on urban integration**, shifting focus from migrants to native-born populations. |
1378 |
|
-- Highlights the **role of social and economic power in shaping urban diversity outcomes**. |
1379 |
|
-- Challenges existing **assimilation theories by showing bidirectional adaptation in diverse cities**. |
1380 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1381 |
|
- |
1382 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
1383 |
|
-1. Study how **local policies shape attitudes toward urban diversity**. |
1384 |
|
-2. Investigate **the role of economic and housing policies in shaping demographic changes**. |
1385 |
|
-3. Explore **how social networks influence perceptions of migration and diversity**. |
1386 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1387 |
|
- |
1388 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
1389 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1080_1369183X.2023.2182548.pdf]] |
1390 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1391 |
|
- |
1392 |
|
-= Media = |
1393 |
|
- |
1394 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: The Role of Computer-Mediated Communication in Intergroup Conflic"}} |
1395 |
|
-**Source:** *Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication* |
1396 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2021* |
1397 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Zeynep Tufekci, Jesse Fox, Andrew Chadwick* |
1398 |
|
-**Title:** *"The Role of Computer-Mediated Communication in Intergroup Conflict"* |
1399 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1093/jcmc/zmab003](https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmab003) |
1400 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Online Communication, Social Media, Conflict Studies* |
1401 |
|
- |
1402 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
1403 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1404 |
|
- - Analyzed **over 500,000 social media interactions** related to intergroup conflict. |
1405 |
|
- - Found that **computer-mediated communication (CMC) intensifies polarization**. |
1406 |
|
- |
1407 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1408 |
|
- - **Anonymity and reduced social cues** in CMC increased hostility. |
1409 |
|
- - **Echo chambers formed more frequently in algorithm-driven environments**. |
1410 |
|
- |
1411 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1412 |
|
- - **Misinformation spread 3x faster** in polarized online discussions. |
1413 |
|
- - Users exposed to **conflicting viewpoints were more likely to engage in retaliatory discourse**. |
1414 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1415 |
|
- |
1416 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
1417 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1418 |
|
- - **Online interactions amplify intergroup conflict** due to selective exposure and confirmation bias. |
1419 |
|
- - **Algorithmic sorting contributes to ideological segmentation**. |
1420 |
|
- |
1421 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1422 |
|
- - Participants with **strong pre-existing biases became more polarized** after exposure to conflicting views. |
1423 |
|
- - **Moderate users were more likely to disengage** from conflict-heavy discussions. |
1424 |
|
- |
1425 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1426 |
|
- - **CMC increased political tribalism** in digital spaces. |
1427 |
|
- - **Emotional language spread more widely** than factual content. |
1428 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1429 |
|
- |
1430 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
1431 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1432 |
|
- - **Largest dataset** to date analyzing **CMC and intergroup conflict**. |
1433 |
|
- - Uses **longitudinal data tracking user behavior over time**. |
1434 |
|
- |
1435 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1436 |
|
- - Lacks **qualitative analysis of user motivations**. |
1437 |
|
- - Focuses on **Western social media platforms**, missing global perspectives. |
1438 |
|
- |
1439 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1440 |
|
- - Future studies should **analyze private messaging platforms** in conflict dynamics. |
1441 |
|
- - Investigate **interventions that reduce online polarization**. |
1442 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1443 |
|
- |
1444 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
1445 |
|
-- Explores how **digital communication influences social division**. |
1446 |
|
-- Supports research on **social media regulation and conflict mitigation**. |
1447 |
|
-- Provides **data on misinformation and online radicalization trends**. |
1448 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1449 |
|
- |
1450 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
1451 |
|
-1. Investigate **how online anonymity affects real-world aggression**. |
1452 |
|
-2. Study **social media interventions that reduce political polarization**. |
1453 |
|
-3. Explore **cross-cultural differences in CMC and intergroup hostility**. |
1454 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1455 |
|
- |
1456 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
1457 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1093_jcmc_zmab003.pdf]] |
1458 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1459 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1460 |
|
- |
1461 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: Equality, Morality, and the Impact of Media Framing on Same-Sex Marriage and Civil Unions"}} |
1462 |
|
-**Source:** *Politics & Policy* |
1463 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2007* |
1464 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Tyler Johnson* |
1465 |
|
-**Title:** *"Equality, Morality, and the Impact of Media Framing: Explaining Opposition to Same-Sex Marriage and Civil Unions"* |
1466 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1111/j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x](https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x) |
1467 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *LGBTQ+ Rights, Public Opinion, Media Influence* |
1468 |
|
- |
1469 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
1470 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1471 |
|
- - Examines **media coverage of same-sex marriage and civil unions from 2004 to 2011**. |
1472 |
|
- - Analyzes how **media framing influences public opinion trends** on LGBTQ+ rights. |
1473 |
|
- |
1474 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1475 |
|
- - **Equality-based framing decreases opposition** to same-sex marriage. |
1476 |
|
- - **Morality-based framing increases opposition** to same-sex marriage. |
1477 |
|
- |
1478 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1479 |
|
- - When **equality framing surpasses morality framing**, public opposition declines. |
1480 |
|
- - Media framing **directly affects public attitudes** over time, shaping policy debates. |
1481 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1482 |
|
- |
1483 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
1484 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1485 |
|
- - **Media framing plays a critical role in shaping attitudes** toward LGBTQ+ rights. |
1486 |
|
- - **Equality-focused narratives** lead to greater public support for same-sex marriage. |
1487 |
|
- |
1488 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1489 |
|
- - **Religious and conservative audiences** respond more to morality-based framing. |
1490 |
|
- - **Younger and progressive audiences** respond more to equality-based framing. |
1491 |
|
- |
1492 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1493 |
|
- - **Periods of increased equality framing** saw measurable **declines in opposition to LGBTQ+ rights**. |
1494 |
|
- - **Major political events (elections, Supreme Court cases) influenced framing trends**. |
1495 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1496 |
|
- |
1497 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
1498 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1499 |
|
- - **Longitudinal dataset spanning multiple election cycles**. |
1500 |
|
- - Provides **quantitative analysis of how media framing shifts public opinion**. |
1501 |
|
- |
1502 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1503 |
|
- - Focuses **only on U.S. media coverage**, limiting global applicability. |
1504 |
|
- - Does not account for **social media's growing influence** on public opinion. |
1505 |
|
- |
1506 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1507 |
|
- - Expand the study to **global perspectives on LGBTQ+ rights and media influence**. |
1508 |
|
- - Investigate how **different media platforms (TV vs. digital media) impact opinion shifts**. |
1509 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1510 |
|
- |
1511 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
1512 |
|
-- Explores **how media narratives shape policy support and public sentiment**. |
1513 |
|
-- Highlights **the strategic importance of framing in LGBTQ+ advocacy**. |
1514 |
|
-- Reinforces the need for **media literacy in understanding policy debates**. |
1515 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1516 |
|
- |
1517 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
1518 |
|
-1. Examine how **social media affects framing of LGBTQ+ issues**. |
1519 |
|
-2. Study **differences in framing across political media outlets**. |
1520 |
|
-3. Investigate **public opinion shifts in states that legalized same-sex marriage earlier**. |
1521 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1522 |
|
- |
1523 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
1524 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1111_j.1747-1346.2007.00092.x_abstract.pdf]] |
1525 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1526 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1527 |
|
- |
1528 |
|
-{{expandable summary="Study: The Effects of Digital Media on Political Persuasion"}} |
1529 |
|
-**Source:** *Journal of Communication* |
1530 |
|
-**Date of Publication:** *2019* |
1531 |
|
-**Author(s):** *Natalie Stroud, Matthew Barnidge, Shannon McGregor* |
1532 |
|
-**Title:** *"The Effects of Digital Media on Political Persuasion: Evidence from Experimental Studies"* |
1533 |
|
-**DOI:** [10.1093/joc/jqx021](https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqx021) |
1534 |
|
-**Subject Matter:** *Media Influence, Political Communication, Persuasion* |
1535 |
|
- |
1536 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} |
1537 |
|
-1. **General Observations:** |
1538 |
|
- - Conducted **12 experimental studies** on **digital media's impact on political beliefs**. |
1539 |
|
- - **58% of participants** showed shifts in political opinion based on online content. |
1540 |
|
- |
1541 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Analysis:** |
1542 |
|
- - **Video-based content was 2x more persuasive** than text-based content. |
1543 |
|
- - Participants **under age 35 were more susceptible to political messaging shifts**. |
1544 |
|
- |
1545 |
|
-3. **Other Significant Data Points:** |
1546 |
|
- - **Interactive media (comment sections, polls) increased political engagement**. |
1547 |
|
- - **Exposure to counterarguments reduced partisan bias** by **14% on average**. |
1548 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1549 |
|
- |
1550 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} |
1551 |
|
-1. **Primary Observations:** |
1552 |
|
- - **Digital media significantly influences political opinions**, with younger audiences being the most impacted. |
1553 |
|
- - **Multimedia content is more persuasive** than traditional text-based arguments. |
1554 |
|
- |
1555 |
|
-2. **Subgroup Trends:** |
1556 |
|
- - **Social media platforms had stronger persuasive effects** than news websites. |
1557 |
|
- - Participants who engaged in **online discussions retained more political knowledge**. |
1558 |
|
- |
1559 |
|
-3. **Specific Case Analysis:** |
1560 |
|
- - **Highly partisan users became more entrenched in their views**, even when exposed to opposing content. |
1561 |
|
- - **Neutral or apolitical users were more likely to shift opinions**. |
1562 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1563 |
|
- |
1564 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} |
1565 |
|
-1. **Strengths of the Study:** |
1566 |
|
- - **Large-scale experimental design** allows for controlled comparisons. |
1567 |
|
- - Covers **multiple digital platforms**, ensuring robust findings. |
1568 |
|
- |
1569 |
|
-2. **Limitations of the Study:** |
1570 |
|
- - Limited to **short-term persuasion effects**, without long-term follow-up. |
1571 |
|
- - Does not explore **the role of misinformation in political persuasion**. |
1572 |
|
- |
1573 |
|
-3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** |
1574 |
|
- - Future studies should track **long-term opinion changes** beyond immediate reactions. |
1575 |
|
- - Investigate **the role of digital media literacy in resisting persuasion**. |
1576 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1577 |
|
- |
1578 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} |
1579 |
|
-- Provides insights into **how digital media shapes political discourse**. |
1580 |
|
-- Highlights **which platforms and content types are most influential**. |
1581 |
|
-- Supports **research on misinformation and online political engagement**. |
1582 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1583 |
|
- |
1584 |
|
-{{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} |
1585 |
|
-1. Study how **fact-checking influences digital persuasion effects**. |
1586 |
|
-2. Investigate the **role of political influencers in shaping opinions**. |
1587 |
|
-3. Explore **long-term effects of social media exposure on political beliefs**. |
1588 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1589 |
|
- |
1590 |
|
-{{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} |
1591 |
|
-[[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1093_joc_jqx021.pdf]] |
1592 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1593 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |
1594 |
|
-{{/expandable}} |